harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (02/23/91)
PSYCOLOQUY (ISSN 1055-0143) Fri, 22 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 3 New Electronic Forum: EduTel New Journal: Psychology Teaching Review Second Call for Papers : Language Origins Society NIPS-91 Call for Workshop Proposals ---------------------------------------------------------------------- From: Arnie Kahn <FAC_ASKAHN%JMUVAX1@pucc> Subject: New Electronic Forum: EduTel EduTel: A Forum for Considering CMC Applications in Educational Contexts As the international academic community becomes increasingly familiar with computer networking, there has been widespread interest in understanding how this new communication medium can be used in educational contexts. Since the networks themselves may be the best medium for discussing educational applications, a cross-disciplinary, cross-institutional group of scholars has joined forces to create and guide EduTel, an electronic conference whose purpose is to consider how computer-mediated communication can be used to achieve educational goals. Initially, EduTel staff will encourage discussion related to three broad educational themes. One theme will focus on how computer- mediated communication can enhance students' and faculty's educational experiences within and outside the classroom contexts. A second theme will address how CMC can improve the education and life style of the physically and educationally handicapped. A third theme will be international development: specifically, how developing nations are using CMC to foster education and research within and between themselves. These themes will constitute three separate but simultaneous lines of discussion available to conference participants. Each of these lines of discussion will be moderated and guided by one or two conference staff members. It will also be possible to pose questions or introduce issues that are not related to the themes defined above. As the conference continues, we expect new themes to emerge and, given sufficient interest, be incorporated into ongoing discussion. EduTel is scheduled to begin on Monday, February 11, 1991 and continue into May, 1991. Those interested in participating can subscribe by either: (a) sending an interactive message to COMSERVE@RPIECS.bitnet with the following command: Subscribe EduTel First_Name Last_Name (Example:) Subscribe EduTel Mary Smith (b) sending this same command (with no other punctuation or words) in the message portion of an electronic mail message addressed to either: Comserve@Rpiecs (Bitnet) or Comserve@Vm.Ecs.Rpi.Edu (Internet) Further information about the EduTel conference will be sent to subscribers when the conference begins. Coordinating Moderator: Norman Coombs Rochester Institute of Technology CMC and the Classroom: Boyd Davis University of North Carolina at Charlotte Don Ploch University of Tennessee -- Knoxville CMC and Disability: Bob Zenhausern St. Johns University CMC and International Development: Sam Lanfranco York University CMC and Other Educational Questions: Karen O'Quin Buffalo State College Technical Moderator: Teresa Harrison Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute ------------------------------ From: J. Hartley <psa04%uk.ac.keele.seq1@pucc> Subject: New Journal: Psychology Teaching Review There is to be a new journal called Psychology Teaching Review published in the U.K. by the British Psychological Society`s group of teachers of psychology. The journal is to be published twice yearly, commencing in March 1992. Details are available from the editor, Stephen Newstead, Dept of Psychology, Polytechnic South West, Drake circus, Plymouth PL4 8AA, U.K. or E-mail p02111@uk.ac.poly-south-west.prime-a. janet. ------------------------------ From: TB0EXC1@NIU.BITNET Subject: Second Call for Papers : Language Origins Society 7th Annual Meeting July 18-20 1991 Northern Illinois University DeKalb Il 60115 USA The Language Origins Society invites abstracts for papers on aspects of language origins and evolution. Language Origins is construed very broadly and includes investigations into the philosophical, neurological, biological or social bases of the phylogeny and/or ontogeny of language in any of its forms (speech, writing, sign) or the social and/or linguistic bases of language evolution and change. Possible topics include (but are not limited to) the origins and development of: phonetic systems grammatical systems semantic systems writing systems speech and language biological, neurological and medical aspects non-human communication systems particular language families and subfamilies pidgin and creole languages Send abstracts of 500 words or less and requests for further information to: Edward Callary Coordinator, LOS English Department Northern Illinois University DeKalb Il 60115 USA e-mail: TB0EXC1@NIU.BITNET (TB ZERO, not the letter O) FAX:815-753-1824 TELEPHONE: 815-753-0611 Deadline for receipt of abstracts is 10 March, 1991 Promising abstracts from advanced students are especially welcome. ------------------------------------------------------------- From: jcp%sarnoff.com@pucc (John Pearson W343 x2385) Subject: NIPS-91 Workshop CALL FOR WORKSHOP Proposals NIPS*91 Post-Conference Workshops December 6 and 7, 1991 Vail, Colorado Following the regular NIPS program, workshops on current topics on Neural Information Processing will be held on December 6 and 7, 1991, in Vail, Colorado. Proposals by qualified individuals interested in chairing one of these workshops are solicited. Past topics have included: Rules and Connectionist Models; Speech; Vision; Sensory Biophysics; Neural Network Dynamics; Neurobiology; Computational Complexity Issues; Fault Tolerance in Neural Networks; Benchmarking and Comparing Neural Network Applications; Architectural Issues; Fast Training Techniques; Control; Optimization, Statistical Inference, Genetic Algorithms; VLSI and Optical Implementations; Integration of Neural Networks with Conventional Software. The format of the workshop is informal. Beyond reporting on past research, the goal is to provide a forum for scientists actively working in the field to freely discuss current issues of concern and interest. Sessions will meet in the morning and in the afternoon of both days, with free time in between for the ongoing individual exchange or outdoor activities. Specific open and/or controversial issues are encouraged and preferred as workshop topics. Individuals interested in chairing a workshop must propose a topic of current interest and must be willing to accept responsibility for their group's discussion. Discussion leaders' responsibilities include: arrange brief informal presentations by experts working on this topic, moderate or lead the discussion, and report its high points, findings and conclusions to the group during evening plenary sessions, and in a short (2 page) summary. Submission Procedure: Interested parties should submit a short proposal for a workshop of interest by May 17, 1991. Proposals should include a title and a short description of what the workshop is to address and accomplish. It should state why the topic is of interest or controversial, why it should be discussed and what the targeted group of participants is. In addition, please send a brief resume of the prospective workshop chair, list of publications and evidence of scholarship in the field of interest. Mail submissions to: Dr. Gerald Tesauro, Co-Chair, Attn: NIPS91 Workshops, IBM Research P.O. Box 704 Yorktown Heights, NY 10598 USA Name, mailing address, phone number, and e-mail net address (if applicable) must be on all submissions. Workshop CoChairs: G. Tesauro & S. Kirkpatrick, IBM PROPOSALS MUST BE RECEIVED BY MAY 17,1991 ------------------------------ End of PSYCOLOQUY Digest ******************************
harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (02/23/91)
PSYCOLOQUY (ISSN 1055-0143) Fri, 22 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 3 Animal Research Information Board Paper available: Visual Orientation Multiplexing Cog Sci 1991 Announcement. Psychology Software News --------------------------------------------------------------------- From: Ron Wood <WOOD@MVAX.MED.NYU.EDU> Subscribe to the ANIMAL RESEARCH INFORMATION BOARD The ANIMAL RESEARCH INFORMATION BOARD -- an electronic bulletin board available only through BITNET or INTERNET -- will have information and items of interest on the use of animals in research. It will include the latest informa- tion on: o activities in Congress (legislation and hearings), the Federal agencies, and the States; o Federal regulations; o pro- and anti- animal research groups; o campus activities, and more! This is a moderated board and material for possible posting will be sent to an editor. Those who have access to BITNET or INTERNET can sign on to this service by sending a mail message to: LISTSERV@GWUVM.bitnet with the following command in the body of the text: SUBSCRIBE APAARIB your name For more advice on how to issue such a command, consult your university computer technical assistance staff. It other problems exist, contact Science Directorate staff member Elizabeth Baldwin through e-mail at: APASDEAB@GWUVM or by phone at (202)955-7653. American Psychological Association Science Directorate 1200 17th St. NW Washington, DC 20036 ----------------------------------------------------------------- From: Jonathan Marshall <marshall@cs.unc.edu> Subject: Paper available -- visual orientation multiplexing **** Please do not re-post to other bboards. **** Papers available, hardcopy only. ADAPTIVE NEURAL METHODS FOR MULTIPLEXING ORIENTED EDGES Jonathan A. Marshall Department of Computer Science University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3175 Edge linearization operators are often used in computer vision and in neural network models of vision to reconstruct noisy or incomplete edges. Such operators gather evidence for the presence of an edge at various orientations across all image locations and then choose the orientation that best fits the data at each point. One disadvantage of such methods is that they often function in a winner-take-all fashion: the presence of only a single orientation can be represented at any point; multiple edges cannot be represented where they intersect. For example, the neural Boundary Contour System of Grossberg and Mingolla implements a form of winner-take-all competition between orthogonal orientations at each spatial location, to promote sharpening of noisy, uncertain image data. But that competition may produce rivalry, oscillation, instability, or mutual suppression when intersecting edges (e.g., a cross) are present. This "cross problem" exists for all techniques, including Markov Random Fields, where a representation of a chosen favored orientation suppresses representations of alternate orientations. A new adaptive technique, using both an inhibitory learning rule and an excitatory learning rule, weakens inhibition between neurons representing poorly correlated orientations. It may reasonably be assumed that neurons coding dissimilar orientations are less likely to be coactivated than neurons coding similar orientations. Multiplexing by superposition is ordinarily generated: combinations of intersecting edges become represented by simultaneous activation of multiple neurons, each of which represents a single supported oriented edge. Unsupported or weakly supported orientations are suppressed. The cross problem is thereby solved. [to appear in Proceedings of the SPIE Conference on Advances in Intelligent Systems, Boston, November 1990.] Also available: J.A. Marshall, "A Self-Organizing Scale-Sensitive Neural Network." In Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, San Diego, June 1990, Vol.III., pp.649-654. J.A. Marshall, "Self-Organizing Neural Networks for Perception of Visual Motion." Neural Networks, 3, pp.45-74 (1990). = Jonathan A. Marshall marshall@cs.unc.edu = ---------------------------------------------------------------- From: Kris Hammond <kris@gargoyle.uchicago.edu> Subject: Cog Sci 1991 Announcement. CLARIFICATION OF THE CALL FOR PAPERS The Thirteenth Annual Meeting of The Cognitive Science Society August 7th - 10th 1991 Chicago, Illinois In order to avoid overlapping the submission date for the Machine Learning Conference and give authors as much time as possible to prepare their papers, we have extended the deadline for Cognitive Science 1991 such that papers must be RECEIVED BY MARCH 18TH, 1991. We are also requesting that papers be limited to eight (8) pages to better approximate their final camera ready form. Authors should submit five (5) copies of their papers in hard copy form. All paper submissions should be sent to: Cognitive Science 1991 Department of Computer Science University of Chicago 1100 East 58th Street Chicago, IL 60637 Papers will be accepted either for presentation as talks or posters. Authors are encouraged to submit new work to the poster sessions. They provide a less formal forum for introducing work in its early stages. Length: Papers should be 8 pages long (excluding title page); have 1 inch margins on top, bottom, and sides; and use no smaller than 10pt type. Camera ready versions of papers will be required only after authors are notified of acceptance. Title Page: Each copy of the paper must include a title page, separate from the body of the paper. This should contain: 1. Title of the paper. 2. Full names, postal addresses, phone numbers, and email addresses (if available) of all authors. 3. An abstract of 100-200 words. 4. The area/subarea in which the paper should be reviewed. 5. A note stating whether the first author is a student and should thus be considered for the David Marr award. Time Table: In order to avoid overlapping the submission date for the Machine Learning Conference, we have extended the deadline for Cognitive Science such that papers must be RECEIVED BY MARCH 18TH, 1991. Notification of acceptance or rejection will be made on or before April 24th, 1991 with camera ready versions of papers due May 20th. Symposia: Symposium proposals should include a single page describing the issue or issues that the panel will address, followed by a page for each participant describing relevant work. In general, we recommend that individuals interested in chairing symposia contact the Program Chairs as soon as possible so that details of personnel and content can be discussed. Videotape/Computer Presentations: Videotape and computer presentations will be made during the poster sessions. We are interested in presentations that demonstrate results in the areas of: Artificial Intelligence, Protocol analysis, Educational tools, and Cognitive Ethnographic studies. Authors should submit one copy of a videotape of 15 minutes maximum duration, accompanied by a submission letter that includes: Title; names, addresses, phone numbers, and email addresses of all authors; three copies of an abstract of one to two pages in length. Researchers wishing to present computer demonstrations should contact Kristian Hammond as soon as possible to discuss review criteria and ensure machine availability. Contact: Kristian Hammond Department of Computer Science University of Chicago 1100 East 58th Street Chicago, IL 60637 Dedre Gentner Department of Psychology Northwestern University Evanston, IL 60201 Phone: (312) 702-1571 Electronic Mail: cogsci91@gargoyle.uchicago.edu ----------------------------------------------------------------- From: PSYC4%VAXA.YORK.AC.UK Subject: Psychology Software News ANNOUNCING A PUBLICATION FOR TEACHERS OF PSYCHOLOGY *** PSYCHOLOGY SOFTWARE NEWS *** A newsletter for Psychologists with a concern for using computers in teaching. Psychology Software News is published in the UK by the Computers in Teaching Initiaitve (CTI) Centre for Psychology with the support of a Government grant. The publication is now being made available internationally to promote a wider discussion of the use of educational technology in psychology teaching. Psychology Software News provides information on the use of computers in the teaching of psychology and offers a forum for discussion about the latest developments in information technology and educational innovation. The Newsletter is less formal than Journals such as Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, although it does include a section for peer-reviewed articles. Psychology Software News is published three times a year. The editors welcome submitted articles or items for review. Regular features of the Newsletter include articles on computers and teaching practice, on educational innovation and on software development as well as reviews of software. Editors: Nick Hammond, University of York, UK Annie Trapp, University of York, UK International advisory board Geoff Cumming, La Trobe University, Australia Tom Hewett, Drexel University, USA Walter Schneider, Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh University, USA Subscription Information: Chris Jardine CTI Centre for Psychology University of York York YO1 5DD UK Email: CTIPSYCH@YORK.AC.UK Phone: +44(904)433156 Fax: +44(904)432917 --------------------------------------------------------------- End of PSYCOLOQUY Digest ******************************
harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (02/23/91)
PSYCOLOQUY ISSN 1055-0143 Fri, 22 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 3 Editorial Note: Refereed discussion and ISSN Number Partial Least Squares: Fred L. Bookstein ---------------------------------------------------------------------- Editorial Note: The following paper was refereed by 2 members of PSYCOLOQUY's editorial board and is now open to peer discussion. Commentaries will be posted separately, under the same topic header as this one (Partial Least Squares/Bookstein) for those who wish to follow or contribute to the discussion on this topic. All submissions will be refereed. Articles and commentaries appearing in PSYCOLOQUY may be cited just as paper journal articles are (author, year, title, journal, volume), but in place of page numbers authors should give the date and indicate that it is an electronic journal. Note that PSYCOLOQUY now has a Library of Congress ISSN number and is officially a journal. For now, authors retain exclusive copyright of their contributions because copyright details still have to be clarified in the electronic medium. PSYCOLOQUY back issues are archived and electronically retrievable by anonymous ftp from princeton.edu in directory /pub/harnad ------------------------------------------------------------------- From: Fred_L._Bookstein@um.cc.umich.edu Subject: Paper: Partial Least Squares Partial Least Squares: A Dose-Response Model for Measurement in the Behavioral and Brain Sciences In this brief note I would make use of the open invitation from the Psycoloquy editors to report on a "new," or, rather, newly formalized method for analysis of data from certain behavioral/brain research designs. I believe the approach is worth considering for a much broader range of studies than those in which it has been exploited so far. The technique I shall introduce is usually called "Partial Least Squares," or PLS. It is a variant of a family of least-squares models of correlation matrices introduced in the 1920's by the biometrician Sewall Wright (1889-1988) to link path analysis with factor analysis. The technique was rediscovered, and the present name assigned, by the Swedish econometrician Herman Wold, in diverse sociological applications throughout the 1970's; but the explanation that follows is not Wold's. Also, this version of PLS should not be confused with another algorithm of the same name that applies to univariate prediction and classification problems in chemometrics, an algorithm having another Wold for inventor (Svante Wold, Herman's son). The present method was worked out in application to neurobehavioral sequelae of prenatal exposure to alcohol in 500 children exposed at levels milder than those bringing on frank Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS). Prof. Ann Streissguth of the University of Washington at Seattle has been Principal Investigator of this project since 1973. The reading- list at the end of this note includes expositions of this exemplary analysis at various levels of technical difficulty. Here I have space only for terse overviews of the method under five rubrics: its scientific context, requisite data, computations, interpretation, and comparisons with alternate approaches. 1. Scientific context. PLS is designed for studies of cause and effect in systems under indirect observation. These are not studies of "normal variation." Instead, typically the investigator is trying to extend into the range of human observational studies a pure dose- response nexus known to lead to unipolar syndromes in high-dose cases. In our alcohol example, FAS is known to exist and to be caused by prenatal exposure to alcohol in sufficient quantity. The subject of dose-response studies is the calibration of effect against cause--of response against dose--in the mildly abnormal case ("social drinking"). 2. Measurements. PLS applies to studies in which cause and effect are each measured variously and redundantly. Our alcohol study includes multiple "soft" measures of the integrated intake of alcohol, peak dose, effect on the mother, and the like, all at two times during pregnancy. The measures of "effect," likewise, include an assortment of nearly five hundred measures of neurobehavioral functions typically found to be altered in the full expression of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome. These outcomes are gathered into "blocks" by child's age (from 1 day to 14 years) and behavioral channel, and the analysis proceeds both separately by blocks and with the outcomes all pooled. 3. Computations. (In the formulas to follow, "@" means "subscript" and "r" means "correlation.") PLS attributes meaning to a two-block data set via one or more pairs of "latent variables" (LV's), each with saliences and scores, according to a tightly regulated least-squares procedure. A LV is a linear combination LV@X=[summation]r(Z,X@i)X@i of the variables X@i of one block (cause or effect) with respect to the prediction of or by another variable Z, either measured or latent. In PLS, the variable Z is itself a latent variable LV@Y=[summation]r(LV@X,Y@j)Y@j referring LV@X to a second block, of Y- variables. The salience of a measure of dose is proportional to its correlation with the LV representing outcome; likewise, the salience of an outcome measure is proportional to its correlation with the LV representing dose. Algebraically, the sentence preceding is sufficient to generate an eigenequation for these saliences. One pair of LVs that results has the highest covariance of any pair (after each vector of saliences is normalized to geometric length unity). The saliences and covariances are computed all at once by the classic singular-value decomposition (SVD) of the cross-correlation or cross-covariance matrix of the measures of dose against the measures of outcome. The PLS model "fits" to the extent that this cross-correlation matrix is of rank one; to that extent, the scores one can compute for the LV's case by case each scale the items of one block in the context of predicting or being predicted by the other. Refinements are available that take nonlinearity of prediction into account in familiar psychometric ways, and there are generalizations for systems of arbitrarily many blocks. It may be helpful to compare this two-block procedure with the more familiar approach of canonical correlations analysis (CCA), which is usually explained as an optimization of the correlation between normalized linear combinations of the two blocks. Interpreting the coefficients of these combinations, however, requires the usual stringent assumptions of multiple regression of either canonical variate upon the variables of the other block. Such assumptions are unlikely to obtain when predictors or outcomes are intentionally redundant. (In a typical analysis from the Seattle study, alcohol versus 11 IQ subscores, the first three pairs of canonical variates have nearly the same high correlation; but each involves an uninterpretable contrast among the alcohol variables, and none bears much predictively usable covariance.) In contrast, the PLS procedure begins with the assignment of an interpretive meaning to each coefficient, as being proportional to correlation with the facing LV. From this follows the optimization of covariance of the normalized LV's. In this, PLS directly generalizes the meaning of the coefficients of a principal component (the linear combination LV@X satisfying the definition of LV with Z=LV@X itself), while CCA generalizes only the variance-optimizing property of the same principal component. 4. Interpretation. In a good unidimensional analysis, such as we find for the effect of alcohol measures upon neurobehavioral outcomes, the LV scores may be used to detect high-dose and high-deficit children and to search for covariates that may exacerbate or attenuate the effect of dose. Furthermore, the saliences can be sorted, block by block, to suggest rosters that are particularly sensitive (by virtue of causal relevance or careful measurement) or particularly insensitive (by virtue of causal irrelevance or irreducible measurement imprecision) to the dose-response relation under study. In the alcohol example, measures of binge drinking very early in pregnancy are the most salient aspects of dose; the salient outcomes include measures of arithmetic skills, attention, and many others. Because two-block PLS is effectively a principal-components analysis of either the rows or the columns of the cross-block correlation matrix, its pathologies are the milder ones characteristic of PCA (influential observations, clusters) rather than those of multiple regression or likelihood-based modeling of covariance structures. The "data" for the PCA are correlations rather than individual measurements, further ameliorating these difficulties. 5. Other statistical methods for indirect studies of neurobehavioral phenomena in humans. PLS may be contrasted with diverse other approaches to the same sort of data. By maximizing covariance between the LV scores, PLS optimizes the usefulness of the analysis for subsequent studies of intervention. Unlike the coefficients of a canonical correlations analysis, the saliences PLS computes have meaning individually even when (indeed, especially when) the predictor block or the outcome block is intentionally multicollinear. Along with the scores, the saliences can be computed in any statistical package that has a principal component feature, so that PLS can be applied to vastly larger problems than can more sophisticated optimizations. PLS differs from structural equations models in its lack of most distributional assumptions and in that it invariably ignores the within-block factor structure of the dose measures and the response measures separately. In our experience, this structure is quite irrelevant to the assigned task of cross-block explanation. (For instance, alcohol doesn't affect the general factor of IQ as much as it affects a particular profile of arithmetic deficiency.) As a fit to the cross-correlation matrix rather than the raw data, PLS avoids the difficulty of all likelihood-based structural equation modeling (including multiple regression) that to be interpretable a fitted model must first be "true." While PLS is not designed for the "testing" of "hypotheses," the usual exploratory resampling data analyses can be applied to substantive aspects of the interpretations that result under (4), such as covariates of LV scores or the reliable identification of types of dose or response measures as particularly salient for each other. To date this mode of PLS has been applied in diverse evolutionary and developmental studies as well as in the extensive study of alcohol effects to which I've been referring. Many more studies of behavioral/ brain development could be cast into a framework for which these simple computations, and the insights they support, might be very useful. This version of PLS was designed to reward careful, conscientious measurement of multiple aspects of familiar but only indirectly observable phenomena and to discourage all modeling that drifts farther than necessary from such data. I would welcome comments from readers, whatever their discipline, regarding precursors of this technique, other potential applications, or pitfalls. For further reading: A. On this dose-response form of PLS: Streissguth, A., H. Barr, F. L. Bookstein, and P. Sampson. Neurobehavioral effects of prenatal alcohol. Neurotoxicology and Teratology 11:461-507, 1989. Ketterlinus, R. D., Fred L. Bookstein, P. Sampson, and M. Lamb. Partial Least Squares analysis in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology 1:351-371, 1989. Bookstein, Fred L., P. D. Sampson, A. P. Streissguth, and H. M. Barr. Measuring "dose" and "response" with multivariate data using Partial Least Squares techniques. Communications in Statistics: Theory and Methods 19:765-804, 1990. B. Two readers in earlier styles of PLS analysis: Joreskog, K. G., and H. Wold, eds. Systems Under Indirect Observation: Causality, Structure, Prediction. Contributions to Economic Analysis, Volume 139, Part II. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1982. Wold, H., ed. Theoretical Empiricism: A General Rationale for Scientific Model Building. New York: Paragon House, 1989. Fred L. Bookstein Center for Human Growth The University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-0406 Fred_L._Bookstein@UM.CC.UMICH.EDU ------------------------------ PSYCOLOQUY is sponsored by the Science Directorate of the American Psychological Association (202) 955-7653 Co-Editors: (scientific discussion) (professional/clinical discussion) Stevan Harnad Perry London, Dean, Cary Cherniss (Assoc Ed.) Psychology Department Graduate School of Applied Graduate School of Applied Princeton University and Professional Psychology and Professional Psychology Rutgers University Rutgers University Assistant Editors: Malcolm Bauer John Pizutelli Psychology Department Psychology Department Princeton University Rutgers University End of PSYCOLOQUY Digest ******************************
harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (02/23/91)
PSYCOLOQUY (ISSN 1055-0143) Fri, 22 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 3 Applied Behavior Analysis Position, Queens College, New York Neuroscience Technical Assistant, Newfoundland ---------------------------------------------------------------------- From: Bruce L. Brown <BROWN@QCVAX.BITNET> Subject: Applied Behavior Analysis Position, Queens College, CUNY APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS POSITION The Department of Psychology at Queens College of the City University of New York anticipates a tenure-track position at the assistant or associate professor level for an applied behavior analyst starting September 1991. Salary range is from $28,630 - $58,129 depending upon qualifications and experience. In addition to teaching at the undergraduate and graduate levels, the successful candidate will be expected to develop an extensive program of applied research that will incorporate data-based Masters- and Doctoral- student field supervision in Applied Behavior Analysis. A Ph.D. degree is required, a focus on developmental disabilities is desirable, and experience in field-research supervision is essential. The candidate will be considered for membership in the Learning Processes Subprogram of CUNY's Doctoral Program in Psychology, and Queens College's Master's Degree Program in Clinical Applications in Mental Health Settings. Applicants should submit a letter of application, a curriculum vitae, sample publications, and three letters of recommendation to: Applied Behavior Analysis Search Committee, Department of Psychology, Queens College/CUNY, 65-30 Kissena Blvd., Flushing, NY 11367-0904. Applications will be reviewed upon receipt and will be accepted until the position is filled. AA/EOE. ------------------------------ From: mzagorsk%kean.ucs.mun.ca@pucc Subject: Neuroscience Technical Assistant, Newfoundland NEUROSCIENCE TECHNICAL ASSISTANT ZZee Ltd. is a small R&D firm located in St. John's, Newfoundland looking for a creative multi-talented individual with a the minimum of a B.Sc. in math and physics or engineering, and a strong background in the neurosciences. This person must have a demonstrable track record in the design, construction, testing, and pre-producion protoyping of laboatory instrumentation or health care products. A thorough competence in digital and analogue hardware, and software, and some experience in VLSI is a must. The working environment requires creativity, perseverance the ability to meet deadlines, and to handle a variety of projects. Interested Canadian should write to or call Dr. M. A. Zagorski 82 Old Topsail Road, St John's, Nfld, Canada A1E 2A8, (709) 753-1839, or leave a note indicating interest and background to this E-Mail Address. ------------------------------ End of PSYCOLOQUY Digest ******************************
harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (02/23/91)
PSYCOLOQUY (ISSN 1055-0143) Fri, 22 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 3 correction to Psycoloquy v2 #2 Mon, 4 Feb 91 QUERY: Decision-making under uncertainty QUERY: Psychology programs QUERY: Sex differences in Handwriting QUERY: Psychology Experiments Conducted by Email ---------------------------------------------------------------------- From: "Gordon Becker" <becker@zeus.unomaha.edu> Subject: correction to Psycoloquy v2 #2 Mon, 4 Feb 91 In the the fourth paragraph of my reply to Stodolsky (PSYCOLOQUY Mon, 4 Feb 91 Volume 2 : Issue 2 Consensus Journals/Becker) "Becker" should be changed to "Stodolsky" so that the paragraph reads, Apparently Stodolsky would like readers to believe that stating his GOAL for consensus journals PROVES his methods attain that goal, that my substituting the term "moderator" for his term "mediator" is so distorted that he need not rebut my specific criticisms, and/or that if the mediator does NOT know the identity of the referee than the referee is NOT anonymous. I fail to see the logic in any of these positions. ------------------------------ From: ggordon@nprdc.navy.mil (Gary Gordon) Subject: QUERY: Decision-making under uncertainty I am interested in the cognitive processes (strategies, heuristics, biases) of decision making under uncertainty and/or severely stressful conditions. I am particularly interested in judgements made with limitted information and with harsh time constraints, as might be the case for pilots, radar operators, and other military personnel. Any research, articles or expertise is appreciated. Thanking you in advance. ------------------------------ From: Klas Brenk <klas.brenk%uni-lj.ac.mail.YU> Subject: QUERY: Psychology programs Our psychology department (University of Ljubljana - Slovenia, Yugoslavia) is in the course of modernization of teaching and research programs in all fields of psychology. We have about 200 undergraduate, about 50 graduate students and a faculty of 25. We are well equipped with computers (Vax cluster, work stations /Sun Sparc, 486/, PC-s in LAN). Our library is quite well stocked (about 16.000 book units, 64 journals). At the moment we are trying to set up a computer driven experimental laboratory for basic courses for which we are buying equipment. We are looking for better connections with psychology departments outside Yugoslavia. We are interested in international research projects and other kinds of academic cooperation in various fields of psychology. We would like to adapt our curriculum to european standards. So right now, we would very much like to see your undergraduate and graduate psychology curriculums. We are also interested in other information about psychology studies at your department (e.g. what jobs can be taken immediately after graduation, what supplementary education must be completed for other jobs etc.) Therefore we would appreciate if you'd send us teaching programs of your department, group etc. on different levels of studies. If You are interested, we can send You our curriculum and a list of our research projects with names of researchers. Hope to hear from you and see you in Ljubljana soon. With compliments. Klas Brenk Please, contact us direct: * e-mail: KLAS.BRENK@UNI-LJ.AC.MAIL.YU * fax: +38 61/332-659 dr. Matija Klas BRENK University of Ljubljana Department of Psychology 61 000 LJUBLJANA Yugoslavia (later could be SLOVENIA) ------------------------------ From: J. Hartley <psa04%uk.ac.keele.seq1> Subject: QUERY: Sex differences in Handwriting I have just carried out an experiment which indicated that six to seven year old children could recognise the sex of handwriting by other children of their age, but were not able to imitate it. Does anyone know of any experiments on sex differences in handwriting at this early age, and of any speculations on why the handwriting of boys should be different from that of girls? I would appreciate any references on the matter. Jim Hartley. ------------------------------ From: andrew%calvin.doc.ca (Andrew Patrick) Subject: Psychology Experiments Conducted by Email Suppose that a psychologist designs an experiment that is controlled entirely by a computer. (This is not rare these days, most labs are either entirely or partially computer-controlled.) Let's say it's an experiment on memory in which the computer presents a list of words to remember, then some distracting task for a period of time, and then allows people to type-in all the words they can recall. This kind of experiment has probably been running thousands of times throughout the world. Subjects come into the lab, sit in front of the computer and read instructions on the screen, and then perform the experiment. In fact, some schools have networked computers to support testing groups of subjects in a "computer lab". Now suppose that the subjects do not come to the lab, but instead particpate in the experiment remotely using dial-in lines or international research networks. That is, the psychologist does not set the computer up in a lab and recruit undergraduate subjects, but instead sets up the computer on a network and invites people to connect using "telnet" or a toll-free dial-in line. What are the ethical implications of such an experiment? Let's presume that the psychologist writes the program that controls the experiment such that: - the subjects are informed about the nature of the study at the beginning. - they have an opportunity to quit the experiment at any time. - individual names are not recorded. - de-briefing information is provided at the conclusion of the procedure. - the psychologist provides his name, address, and phone number for people who are interested in learning more about the study or have problems with it. There are some scientific implications of this kind of experiment, including being able to control and monitor what the subjects are actually doing at the other end of the network connection (e.g., are they writing the words down during the study phase), a peculiar sample of subjects (only those with network connections or modems), and subjects participating more than once, but let's assume that the psychologist is willing to live with those factors. The question is: Are there any ethical factors that should be considered in such an experiment? My interest here is not purely theoretical -- I would like to try running such an experiment. Andrew Patrick, Ph.D. Department of Communications, Ottawa, CANADA andrew@calvin.doc.CA ------------------------------------------------------------- PSYCOLOQUY is sponsored by the Science Directorate of the American Psychological Association (202) 955-7653 Co-Editors: (scientific discussion) (professional/clinical discussion) Stevan Harnad Perry London, Dean, Cary Cherniss (Assoc Ed.) Psychology Department Graduate School of Applied Graduate School of Applied Princeton University and Professional Psychology and Professional Psychology Rutgers University Rutgers University Assistant Editors: Malcolm Bauer John Pizutelli Psychology Department Psychology Department Princeton University Rutgers University End of PSYCOLOQUY Digest ******************************