[net.followup] Are you seeing pink walls?

sims@hou2b.UUCP (12/10/83)

I have noticed a similar effect when I was a graduate student
at the U. of Illinois.  After several hours working on an
orange/red plasma PLATO terminal, all whites (particularly
chalk lines on a blackboard) were shifted to blue.
-- 
                                    Jim
                             houxm!hou2b!sims

lat@wbux5.UUCP (12/11/83)

I have noticed the same...I am currently using a Concept AVT
terminal, black background w/orange letters (can use it in
reverse also), and I am also seeing blue writing on the
chalkboard, instead of white.....I don't remember having a
problem like that with my HP2621...I also notice that in the
course of the day my eyes are more tired than they usually
are...could this be caused by the Concept?  I had heard that
amber on black was much easier on the eyes...is orange on
black as bad as white (or green, whatever the HP was) on
black?

Laurie
[ihnp4, houxf]wbux5!lat

rfg@hound.UUCP (R.GRANTGES) (12/12/83)

About three centuries ago when I was in school, one of the things
they told you about in introductory Psych was a phenomena (I've
forgotten its name) wherein the eye behaves very much as you all
have been describing.  It can be demonstrated easily by gazing
fixedly at any strongly colored surface, then looking away at
some neutral (grey, white) surface. You will see the second surface
not as grey or white but as the complementary color of the first
surface. Don't know why, but that's how it works. Effect is well
known, don't worry about it - that's my message.
  -Dick Grantges HO   hound!rfg

judd@umcp-cs.UUCP (12/12/83)

.......

Colors of phosphors can be ranked from best to worst thus:
amber (a very specific color), yellow, green, orange, red, blue

This ranking comes from various research done in Europe.  The papers
can be found in a book whose title I have forgotten.  I found it searching
through the card catalog for books on human factors research.

Note that some greens are much worse than others.  The papers quote specific
phosphors.  The P3 used by the IBM monochrome monitor is good.  Whatever
is used by techtronix 4025 is pretty poor.
-- 
Spoken: Judd Rogers
Arpa:   judd.umcp-cs@CSNet-relay
Uucp:...{allegra,seismo}!umcp-cs!judd

lwr@ih1ap.UUCP (L. W. Retzner) (12/12/83)

I have noticed the same phenomenon when wearing rose colored
ski goggles on a bright sun suny day.  When ever I took the
goggles off the snow would turn green for a few seconds.  It
did not take too long for my eyes to readjust or to put the
goggles back on because of brightness.

				Bill Retzner
				ihnp4!ih1ap!lwr

walsh@ihuxi.UUCP (12/13/83)

Your eyes will always 'see' the complimentary color of anything you've
been looking at for a while, when you look at a white or pale surface.
Such as when you stare at a blue (bright) image for a few seconds, and then
look at the white cieling, you'll see an orange 'shadow' that is the same
shape as the blue image. Green and red are complimentary, and the image is
pink because of the brightness and light value of the green on terminal
screens. I have a green on black screen, and I'm always seeing pink! I don't
know the exact medical reasons for this reverse imaging; maybe the cones
in the retina have to compensate with a negative image.
  When I had a blue on black screen, I never noticed this (i.e., I didn't
see orange), but I think that's becasuse the blue used is more like a grey,
while green screens are REALLY green.

B. Walsh

zben@umcp-cs.UUCP (12/13/83)

Last time I looked the theory was that there are three different pigments
in the cones (color-vision cells) of the eye.  I suspect one of the pigments
or it's associated neural net elements become fatigued, so that when you
switch to a white or neutral stimulation the output is not quite balanced,
and a perception of a pale color results...

Ben Cranston         ...seismo!umcp-cs!zben       zben@umd2.ARPA

daemon@decwrl.UUCP (12/14/83)

From: Joe Falcone HLO2-3/C09 dtn: 225-6059 <erlang::falcone>
CC:	 


I don't claim to be an expert on the matter, but a a year ago I spent
a few months studying color vision (in particular, the perception of
color vs. black-and-white images on computer displays), and the one thing
that amazed me about the field was how little we know for certain about
the mechanisms of color vision.  There are many theories, but very little
evidence (since that would involve possibly destructive experiments).
The following is a capsule of some theories.

The theory proposed by Rick Wise is off the mark to begin with.  The human
eye does not work like a TV camera - and in fact few eyes in nature do.
Instead of red, yellow, blue and intensity receptors, the eye has red
and green receptors for bright light, intensity receptors for dim light,
and there is some debate as to whether we have blue receptors at all
(there are some visual tricks which can be explained by that).

Hence, a workable theory (and one that I believe has been supported by
some laboratory tests) is that under strong influence, a receptor can
be suppressed, so that when the stimulus is removed, the other (or
opponent) receptor begins showing a response that is greater relative
to the other one.  SO the green receptors are suppressed, and when the
stimulus is removed, they shut off MUCH MORE than the red receptors do
(since they are "exhausted" from the stimulus), so that the red receptors
are providing relatively more input than the green receptors.
Voila - Pink Walls!

If anyone out there has a red phosphor terminal, I'd be interested in
knowing whether the walls appear green after extended use.  Notice also
that this explains why users of blue phosphor terminals don't see orange
or red/green walls - blue receptors are not that important to color vision
and blue tends to stimulate/suppress red and green receptors equally.

Joe Falcone
decvax!decwrl!rhea!erlang!falcone
DEC Research, Hudson, MA

msimpson@bbncca.ARPA (Mike Simpson) (12/14/83)

**
        Somewhere in the misty eons of my memory, I remember
seeing an optical illusion somewhat similar to this.  There
was a picture of an American flag, with black stars on a
yellow field, and green and black stripes.  One was supposed
to look at this for about a minute, then quickly look at a
white wall.  Voila -- a regular (red-white-blue) American
flag!  The explaination (again, I'm relying on memory) was
that the cones in the back of the retina get tired when you
look at the same color for a period of time, and so the colors
get 'selected' out. 
	Flames to me, comments to the net.
		     Mike Simpson
		     msimpson@bbn-unix (ARPA)
		     {decvax,linus,wjh12}!bbncca!msimpson (Usenet)
-- 
		        -- cheers,
			   Mike Simpson
			   Bolt Beranek and Newman Inc.
			   Ten Moulton Street,
				   Cambridge, MA 02238 (USnail)
			   msimpson@bbn-unix (ARPA)
			   decvax!bbncca!msimpson (Usenet)
			   msimpson.bbn-unix@udel-relay (CSNET)
			   617-497-2819 (Ma Bell)

seifert@ihuxl.UUCP (D.A. Seifert) (12/16/83)

I saw the same reverse US flag. It was in the World Book encyclopedia.
It worked too.

Snoopy
-- 
)
(
 )		from the mildly opinionated keyboard of		
_)__________________	
|OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO|		Dave Seifert
|OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO|		ihnp4!ihuxl!seifert
|OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO|
|------------------|

eds@bunker.UUCP (Eric Schlesinger) (12/19/83)

Not so long ago, the BBC (that's in England, folks) did a series
of Open University programs on such phenomena.  It seems that it
relates primarily to the organization of color receptors in
the human eye, and the fact that when stressed excessively they
fatigue rather rapidly.  This is the case when one stares at a typical
green phosphor for unduely long amounts of time.

Afterwards, the other receptors are out of balance (in terms of 
strength of reception).  So the brain receives an image which is more
heavily excited to the other colors; since it appears that there are
primarily red/green receptors, the green-induced fatigue produces a 
reddish aftereffect.  (Note that most color-blindness is oriented
towards red/green, and that part of a standard eye examination - the
ones I've had, anyway - uses red and green filters to compare visual
acuity.)

I'm sure there's more and more detailed information regarding this
sort of thing somewhere.  But the above was picked up on a half-hour
Open University show; of course, no one has ruled out that after 
staring at a CRT for hours one also has the tendency to drink
excessively which can *also* account for everything having a 
reddish tinge.

Eric Schlesinger
Bunker Ramo Information Systems (known to the trade as BRIS)

eager@amd70.UUCP (01/03/84)

The eye adapts to the predominant color, trying to adjust to a neutral color
balance.  This is why colors seem constant when seen under daylight (at 6000
degrees Kelvin), tungsten light (2500 degrees) or candle (1000 degrees).  That
the reflected light is not the same color can be seen when you take a picture 
indoors with film balanced for daylight, or when the light in a room is a 
mixture of daylight and tungsten light.

When you over-load the eyes with one color, it over-compensates with the 
opposite color.  When you look away, the compensation remains for a time.  If
you look at a sheet of red paper for 30 seconds, and then look at something 
white, you will get an image of the complement, green.  Similarly, with blue
and yellow.  If you look at a pattern of green squares separated by white 
borders, after a short time the borders will tend to look pink, especially
at the corners.