harnad@phoenix.Princeton.EDU (Stevan Harnad) (12/21/90)
(1) Greenfield: Language, Tools and Brain (2) Crain: Language Acquisition in the Absence of Experience (3) Velmans: Is Human Information Processing Unconscious? Below are the abstracts of these three forthcoming target articles, which are to appear in Behavioral and Brain Sciences (BBS), an international, interdisciplinary journal that provides Open Peer Commentary on important and controversial current research in the biobehavioral and cognitive sciences. Commentators must be current BBS Associates or nominated by a current BBS Associate. To be considered as a commentator on this article, to suggest other appropriate commentators, or for information about how to become a BBS Associate, please send email to: harnad@clarity.princeton.edu or harnad@pucc.bitnet or write to: BBS, 20 Nassau Street, #240, Princeton NJ 08542 [tel: 609-921-7771] To help us put together a balanced list of commentators, please give some indication of the aspects of the topic on which you would bring your areas of expertise to bear if you are selected as a commentator. (The articles retrievable by anonymous ftp from directory /pub/harnad on princeton.edu as the files crain.bbs and velmans.bbs [greenfield.bbs not yet there but to be trabsfered soon], however, please do not prepare a commentary unless you have been formally invited to do so.) ____________________________________________________________________ (1) Language, Tools, and Brain: The development and evolution of hierarchically organized sequential behavior Patricia Marks Greenfield Department of Psychology University of California, UCLA Los Angeles, CA 90024-1563 electronic mail: rygreen@uclasscf.bitnet Abstract: During the first two years of life a common neural substrate (roughly, Broca's area) underlies the hierarchically organized combination of elements in the development of both speech and manual action, including tool use. The neural evidence implicates relatively specific cortical circuitry underlying a grammatical "module." Behavioral and neurodevelopmental data suggest that the modular capacities for language and manipulation are not present at birth but come into being gradually during the third and fourth years of life. An evolutionary homologue of the common neural substrate for language production and manual action during the first two years of human life is hypothesized to have provided a foundation for the evolution of language before the divergence of hominids and the great apes. Support comes from the discovery of a Broca's area analogue in contemporary primates. In addition, chimpanzees have an identical constraint on hierarchical complexity in both tool use and symbol combination. Their performance matches that of the two-year-old child who has not yet developed the differentiated neural circuits for the relatively modularized production of complex grammar and complex manual construction activity. ____________________________________________________________________ (2) Language Acquisition in the Absence of Experience Stephen Crain University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories electronic mail: linqadm@uconnvm.bitnet KEYWORDS: acquisition, child language, development, innate competence, grammar, language learnability, parameter theory, maturation, syntactic development, psycholinguistics. ABSTRACT: A fundamental goal of linguistic theory is to explain how natural languages are acquired. This paper describes some recent findings on how learners acquire syntactic knowledge for which there is little, if any, decisive evidence from the environment. The first section presents several general observations about language acquisition that linguistic theory has sought to explain and discusses the thesis that certain linguistic properties are innate because they appear universally and in the absence of corresponding experience. A third diagnostic for innateness, early emergence, is the focus of the second section of the paper, in which linguistic theory is tested against recent experimental evidence on children's acquisition of syntax. ____________________________________________________________________ (3) IS HUMAN INFORMATION PROCESSING CONSCIOUS? Max Velmans Department of Psychology Goldsmiths College University of London electronic mail: MLV@gold.lon.ac.uk KEY WORDS: consciousness, information processing, brain, unconscious, attention, mind, functionalism, reductionism, complementarity. ABSTRACT: Investigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing. Input analysis is thought to be initially "preconscious," "pre-attentive," fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by "conscious," "focal-attentive" analysis which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed to identify complex, novel stimuli. Conscious processing has also been thought to be necessary for choice, learning and memory, and the organization of complex, novel responses, particularly those requiring planning, reflection, or creativity. This target article reviews evidence that consciousness performs none of these functions. Consciousness nearly always results from focal-attentive processing (as a form of output) but does not itself enter into this or any other form of human information processing. This suggests that the term "conscious process" needs re-examination. Consciousness appears to be necessary in a variety of tasks because they require focal-attentive processing; if consciousness is absent, focal-attentive processing is absent. From a first-person perspective, however, conscious states are causally effective. First-person accounts are complementary to third-person accounts. Although they can be translated into third-person accounts, they cannot be reduced to them. Stevan Harnad Department of Psychology Princeton University harnad@clarity.princeton.edu / harnad@pucc.bitnet / srh@flash.bellcore.com harnad@learning.siemens.com / harnad@elbereth.rutgers.edu / (609)-921-7771