[comp.society] Reading and the Aesthetics of Computers

PSY%SEUMDC51.BITNET@CUNYVM.CUNY.EDU (Ake Olofsson) (03/25/88)

Years ago, in the dark northern Swedish winter, I wrote a message to 
one of the directors of the Swedish telephone company, using a 
local computer conferencing system.  It was not a very pleasant 
letter, but urgent.  After a month he replyed and said that the delay 
was caused by malfunctions in his printer.   He had not been able to 
read my letter for several weeks until the printer was fixed.  I did 
not understand why he could not read from a monitor and concluded that 
he was hiding something.

However, later I found out that he simply did not like CRT-reading, 
advocating that it negatively affected his reading comprehension.

Today I know that his represents a very common opinion among ordinary 
users - a lot of people don't like CRT-reading.

Recent discussion in Computers and Society has pointed out a few cases 
where reading from a CRT seems to be favored.  Some people like it and 
some reading tasks are better done on screen.  To a large extent this 
discussion has focused on personal preferences.  These are real and 
important, and I find it very interesting to read the opinions of a 
wide range of computer-users.

But there is also a less subjective side of this question.

There seem to be mainly two reasons for difficulties with CRT-reading. 
The first has to do with the early or peripheral visual stages of 
the reading process.  A lot of research has found that, even for 
highly experienced subjects, proof-reading (quite a familiar task to 
most academics) is slightly slower on standard CRT displays than on 
paper.  The disadvantage with a CRT can be reduced using high resolution 
displays and black characters containing grey level (anti-aliased) on white
background and a font resembling real printing. (See Gould et al., Human 
Factors, Vol 29, No 5). Research in Umea has shown that syntactic errors 
in a text can be spotted more effectively using a CRT. (Jarvella & 
Lundberg, in preparation).

The second factor has to do with our ability to maintain orientation in 
text, which is harder with only 24 lines visible and no spatial, 
tactile information about the rest of the text.  In reading documents 
and books, we have a feeling of where in the text we are.  You can get 
an idea manually how far from the end of the document you are, and 
leave a finger at an important passage while looking ahead.  This 
visual tactile method may not be exact (as indicated by Robert 
Kennedy in Computers and Society Vol3No10) but the important thing is 
that it is cheap, demands little cognitive effort, is nonverbal and 
is fast.   Reading on a computer, on the other hand, usually requires using
commands which may interfere with the actual reading process more than 
the direct physical manipulation of hardcopy does.

Partly,this problem can be compensated for with larger screens, scroll 
bars and a mouse, but text-handling on computers still may put special 
demands on the user's spatial ability (See Vicente et al., Human Factors, 
Vol 29, No 3).

The orientation-in-text problem may explain why we feel uncomfortable in 
reading tasks demanding a full grasp of a text's structure.

Reading tasks can be very different. Reading, comprehending and locating 
information in text are not the same thing (Guthrie & Kirsch, Journal 
of Educational Psychology, Vol 79, p 220). A great deal of the reading 
done in job settings consists in locating information.  When locating 
information, the task is more to remember *where* you read than *what* 
you read, and to compare where you are with respect to your goal and 
decisions needed for the next step.

I think that a better understanding of these differences between reading 
from computer display and hardcopy can help the reader to monitor and 
direct his own reading and in a relaxed way to find a personally 
satisfying solution combining medium and reading task.

Ake Olofsson