[comp.ai] BBS Call for Commentators: 7 target articles

harnad@mind.UUCP (Stevan Harnad) (11/02/87)

Below are the abstracts of seven forthcoming articles on which BBS --
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, an international, interdisciplinary Journal
of Open Peer Commentary, published by Cambridge University Press --
invites self-nominations from potential commentators. The procedure is
explained after the abstracts. The seven articles are:

(1) The Intentional Stance   (Dan Dennett)  [multiple book review]
(2) The Ethological Basis of Learning   (A. Gardner & B. Gardner)
(3) Tactical deception in Primates   (A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne)
(4) Event-Related Potentials and Memory: A Critique of the Context
    Updating Hypothesis   (Rolf Verleger)
(5) Is the P300 Component a Manifestation of Context Updating?
    (E. Donchin & M. Coles)    [article-length precommentary on (4)]
(6) Real and Depicted Spaces: A Cross-Cultural Perspective   (J.B. Deregowski)
(7) Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework (A.W. Logue)

-----

1.                   The Intentional Stance

                        Dan Dennett
                   Philosophy Department
                      Tufts university

The intentional stance is the  strategy  of  prediction  and
explanation  that  attributes  beliefs,  desires  and  other
"intentional" states to organisms and devices  and  predicts
future  behavior from what it would be rational for an agent
to do, given  those  beliefs  and  desires.  Any  device  or
organism   that   regularly   uses   this   strategy  is  an
"intentional system," whatever its  innards  might  be.  The
strategy  of  treating  parts  of  the  world as intentional
systems is the foundation of "folk psychology,"  but  it  is
also  exploited (and is virtually unavoidable) in artificial
intelligence and cognitive science in general, as well as in
evolutionary   theory.  An  analysis  of  the  role  of  the
intentional  stance  and  its  presuppositions  supports   a
naturalistic  theory  of  mental  states  and  events, their
"content" or  "intentionality,"  and  the  relation  between
"mentalistic"  levels  of explanation and neurophysiological
or mechanistic levels of explanation. As such, the  analysis
of  the  intentional stance grounds a theory of the mind and
its relation to the body.

2.           The Ethological Basis of Learning

                  A. Gardner & B. Gardner
                   Psychology Department
                    University of Nevada

One view of the basic nature of  the  learning  process  has
dominated  theory and application throughout the century. It
is the view that the behavior of organisms  is  governed  by
its  positive  and  negative  consequences.  Anyone  who has
attempted to use this principle to teach relatively  complex
skills  to free-living, well-fed subjects -- as we have done
in our sign language studies of chimpanzees  --  is  apt  to
have been disappointed.

Meanwhile, recent ethological  findings  plainly  contradict
the  argument  that most, or even much, of the learning that
takes place in the operant conditioning laboratory is  based
on  the  "law of effect." The residue of support for the law
of effect that might  be derived from  operant  conditioning
experiments  depends  entirely  on the logic of a particular
experimental design. There is, however, a logical defect  in
this  design  that  cannot  be  repaired  by any conceivable
improvement in procedure or instrumentation.  However deeply
ingrained  in  our  cultural  traditions,  the  notion  that
behavior is based on its  positive  consequences  cannot  be
supported  by  laboratory evidence. Several key phenomena of
conditioning can be dealt with  in  a  more  straightforward
manner by dispensing with hedonism altogether,

An  impressive  amount  of  human  behavior  persists,   and
persists  in spite of its negative consequences. The popular
notion that persistent maladaptive behavior is rare in other
animals  is  easily refuted by those who have observed other
animals closely in  their  natural  habitats.  We  offer  an
analysis  of  adaptive  and maladaptive behavior in aversive
conditioning and of the design of experiments on the  effect
of  predictive  contingencies in Pavlovian conditioning. The
latter attempt to demonstrate an effect of contingency fails
because it violates basic principles of experimental design.
We conclude that there is a fundamental  logical  defect  in
all notions of contingency.

This reconsideration of the  traditional  behavioristic  and
cognitive  versions  of  the  law  of  effect was originally
suggested  by  problems  in  teaching  new  and  challenging
patterns   of  behavior  to  free-living  subjects  such  as
children and  chimpanzees,  which  we  briefly  describe  in
closing.

3.             Tactical Deception in Primates

                   A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne
                 Psychological Laboratories
            University of St. Andrews, Scotland

Tactical deception occurs when an individual's  is  able  to
use  an  "honest"  act  from  his  normal  repertoire  in  a
different context to mislead familiar individuals.  Although
primates  have  a  reputation for social skill, most primate
groups are so intimate that any deception is  likely  to  be
subtle  and  infrequent. Records are often anecdotal and not
widely known in the formal literature of behavioral science.
We  have  tackled  this  problem by drawing together records
from many primates and primatologists in order to  look  for
repeating  patterns.  This  has  revealed  a  many  forms of
deceptive  tactics,  which  we  classify  in  terms  of  the
function  they  perform.  For  each  class,  we  sketch  the
features of  another  individual's  state  of  mind  that  a
deceiver  must  be  able  to represent, acting as a "natural
psychologist." Our analysis clarifies and  perhaps  explains
certain  taxonomic differences. Before these findings can be
generalized, however, behavioral scientists  must  agree  on
some fundamental methodological and theoretical questions in
the study of the evolution of social cognition.

4.          Event-Related Potentials and Memory:
       A Critique of the Context Updating Hypothesis

                       Rolf Verleger
                   Mannheim, West Germany

P3 is the most prominent of the electrical potentials of the
human    electroencephalogram    that   are   sensitive   to
psychological variables.  According to the most  influential
current  hypothesis about its psychological significance [E.
Donchin's], the "context updating" hypothesis,  P3  reflects
the  updating  of  working  memory.  This  hypothesis cannot
account for relevant portions of the available evidence  and
it   entails  some  basic  contradictions.  A  more  general
formulation of this  hypothesis  is  that  P3  reflects  the
updating  of  expectancies.  This  version  implies that P3-
evoking stimuli are initially unexpected  but  later  become
expected.  This contradiction cannot be resolved within this
formulation.

The alternative "context  closure"  hypothesis  retains  the
concept  of "strategic information processing" emphasized by
the context updating hypothesis. P3s are  evoked  by  events
that  are awaited when subjects deal with repetitive, highly
structured  tasks;  P3s  arise  from   subjects'   combining
successive  stimuli into larger units The tasks in which P3s
are elicited can accordingly be classified in terms of their
respective   formal  sequences  of  stimuli.  P3  may  be  a
physiological indicator of excess activation being  released
from perceptual control areas.

5. Is the P300 component a manifestation of Context Updating?

          Emanuel Donchin and Michael G. H. Coles
           Cognitive Psychophysiology Laboratory
         University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
[article-length precommentary on Verleger]

To understand the endogenous components of the ERP  we  must
use from data about the components' antecedent conditions to
form hypotheses about the information processing function of
the  underlying  brain activity.  These hypotheses, in turn,
generate testable predictions about the consequences of  the
component. We review the application of this approach to the
analysis  of  the  P300  component,   whose   amplitude   is
controlled  multiplicatively  by  the subjective probability
and the task relevance of the  eliciting  events  and  whose
latency  depends  on  the  duration  of stimulus evaluation.
These  and  other  factors  suggest  that  the  P300  is   a
manifestation  of activity occurring whenever one's model of
the environment must be revised.  Tests of three predictions
based   on  this  "context  updating"  model  are  reviewed.
Verleger's critique is based on a misconstrual of the  model
as  well  as  on  a  partial  and  misleading reading of the
relevant literature.

6.               Real and Depicted Spaces:
                A Cross-Cultural Perspective

                      J.B. Deregowski
                   Psychology Department
              University of Aberdeen, Scotland

This  paper  examines  the  contribution  of  cross-cultural
studies   to   our   understanding  of  the  perception  and
representation of space.  A  cross-cultural  survey  of  the
basic  difficulties  in  understanding  pictures -- from the
failure to recognize a picture as a  representation  to  the
inability  to  recognise the object represented -- indicates
that   similar   difficulties   occur   in   pictorial   and
nonpictorial  cultures.  Real  and  pictorial spaces must be
distinguished. The  experimental  work  on  pictorial  space
derives  from  two distinct traditions: the study of picture
perception  in  "remote"  populations  and  the   study   of
perceptual  illusions.  A  comparison  of  the  findings  on
pictorial  space  perception  with  those  on   real   space
perception  and  perceptual constancies suggests that cross-
cultural differences in the  perception  of  both  real  and
depicted  space involve two different kinds of skills: those
related only to real spaces or only to depicted  spaces  and
those  related  to  both.   Different  cultural  groups  use
different skills to perform the same perceptual task.

7.     Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework

                         A.W. Logue
                  Department of Psychology
                     SUNY - Stony Brook

The tendency to choose a  larger,  more  delayed  reinforcer
over  a  smaller, less delayed one (self-control) depends on
the current physical values  of  the  reinforcers.  It  also
varies  according  to  a  subject's  experience  and current
factors other than the reinforcers.  Two local delay  models
(Mischel's  social learning theory and Herrnstein's matching
law) as well as molar maximization models  have  taken  into
account   these   indirect   effects   on  self  control  by
representing  a  subject's  behavior  as  a  function  of  a
perceived  environment.  A  general evolutionary analysis of
all this  research  yields  a  better  and  more  predictive
description of self control.


-----


This is an experiment in using the Net to find eligible commentators for
articles in Behavioral & Brain Sciences. BBS publishes important and
controversial interdisciplinary articles in psychology, neuroscience, behavioral
biology, cognitive science, artificial intelligence, linguistics and philosophy.
Articles are rigorously refereed and, if accepted, are circulated to a large
number of potential commentators around the world in the various specialties
on which the article impinges. Their 1000-word commentaries are then
co-published with the target article as well as the author's response to each.
The commentaries consist of analyses, elaborations, complementary and
supplementary data and theory, criticisms and cross-specialty syntheses.

Commentators are selected by the following means: (1) BBS maintains a
computerized file of over 3000 BBS Associates; the size of this group is
increased annually as authors, referees, commentators and nominees of current
Associates become eligible to become Associates. Many commentators are selected
from this list. (2) The BBS editorial office does informal as well as formal
computerized literature searches on the topic of the target articles to find
additional potential commentators from across specialties and around the world
who are not yet BBS Associates.  (3) The referees recommend potential
commentators. (4) The author recommends potential commentators.

We now propose to add the following source for selecting potential commentators:
The abstract of the target article will be posted in the relevant newsgroups on
the Net. Eligible individuals who judge that they would have a relevant
commentary to contribute should contact me at the e-mail address indicated at
the bottom of this message, or should write by normal mail to:

Stevan Harnad, Editor, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20 Nassau Street, Room 240
Princeton NJ 08542		 (phone: 609-921-7771)

"Eligibility" usually means being an academically trained professional
contributor to one of the disciplines mentioned earlier, or to related academic
disciplines. The letter should indicate the candidate's general qualifications
as well as their basis for wishing to serve as commentator for the particular
target article in question. It is preferable also to enclose a Curriculum Vitae.
(Please note that the editorial office must exercise selectivity among the
nominations received so as to ensure a strong and balanced cross-specialty
spectrum of eligible commentators.)

This self-nomination format may also be used by those who wish to become BBS
Associates, but they must also specify a current Associate who knows their work
and is prepared to nominate them; where no current Associate is known by the
candidate, the editorial office will send the Vita to approporiate Associates
to ask whether they would be prepared to nominate the candidate.

BBS has rapidly become a widely read and highly influential forum in the
biobehavioral and cognitive sciences. A recent recalculation of BBS's "impact
factor" (ratio of citations to number of articles) in the American Psychologist
[41(3) 1986] reports that already in its fifth year of publication (1982) BBS's
impact factor had risen to become the highest of all psychology journals indexed
as well as 3rd highest of all 1300 journals indexed in the Social Sciences
Citation Index and 50th of all 3900 journals indexed in the Science Citation
index, which indexes all the scientific disciplines.

Potential commentators should send their names, addresses, a description of
their general qualifications and their basis for seeking to comment on this
target article in particular to the address indicated earlier or to the
following e-mail address:	 harnad@mind.princeton.edu
-- 

Stevan Harnad		 harnad@mind.princeton.edu	 (609)4 H0 B229t