harnad@mind.UUCP (Stevan Harnad) (11/02/87)
Below are the abstracts of seven forthcoming articles on which BBS -- Behavioral and Brain Sciences, an international, interdisciplinary Journal of Open Peer Commentary, published by Cambridge University Press -- invites self-nominations from potential commentators. The procedure is explained after the abstracts. The seven articles are: (1) The Intentional Stance (Dan Dennett) [multiple book review] (2) The Ethological Basis of Learning (A. Gardner & B. Gardner) (3) Tactical deception in Primates (A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne) (4) Event-Related Potentials and Memory: A Critique of the Context Updating Hypothesis (Rolf Verleger) (5) Is the P300 Component a Manifestation of Context Updating? (E. Donchin & M. Coles) [article-length precommentary on (4)] (6) Real and Depicted Spaces: A Cross-Cultural Perspective (J.B. Deregowski) (7) Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework (A.W. Logue) ----- 1. The Intentional Stance Dan Dennett Philosophy Department Tufts university The intentional stance is the strategy of prediction and explanation that attributes beliefs, desires and other "intentional" states to organisms and devices and predicts future behavior from what it would be rational for an agent to do, given those beliefs and desires. Any device or organism that regularly uses this strategy is an "intentional system," whatever its innards might be. The strategy of treating parts of the world as intentional systems is the foundation of "folk psychology," but it is also exploited (and is virtually unavoidable) in artificial intelligence and cognitive science in general, as well as in evolutionary theory. An analysis of the role of the intentional stance and its presuppositions supports a naturalistic theory of mental states and events, their "content" or "intentionality," and the relation between "mentalistic" levels of explanation and neurophysiological or mechanistic levels of explanation. As such, the analysis of the intentional stance grounds a theory of the mind and its relation to the body. 2. The Ethological Basis of Learning A. Gardner & B. Gardner Psychology Department University of Nevada One view of the basic nature of the learning process has dominated theory and application throughout the century. It is the view that the behavior of organisms is governed by its positive and negative consequences. Anyone who has attempted to use this principle to teach relatively complex skills to free-living, well-fed subjects -- as we have done in our sign language studies of chimpanzees -- is apt to have been disappointed. Meanwhile, recent ethological findings plainly contradict the argument that most, or even much, of the learning that takes place in the operant conditioning laboratory is based on the "law of effect." The residue of support for the law of effect that might be derived from operant conditioning experiments depends entirely on the logic of a particular experimental design. There is, however, a logical defect in this design that cannot be repaired by any conceivable improvement in procedure or instrumentation. However deeply ingrained in our cultural traditions, the notion that behavior is based on its positive consequences cannot be supported by laboratory evidence. Several key phenomena of conditioning can be dealt with in a more straightforward manner by dispensing with hedonism altogether, An impressive amount of human behavior persists, and persists in spite of its negative consequences. The popular notion that persistent maladaptive behavior is rare in other animals is easily refuted by those who have observed other animals closely in their natural habitats. We offer an analysis of adaptive and maladaptive behavior in aversive conditioning and of the design of experiments on the effect of predictive contingencies in Pavlovian conditioning. The latter attempt to demonstrate an effect of contingency fails because it violates basic principles of experimental design. We conclude that there is a fundamental logical defect in all notions of contingency. This reconsideration of the traditional behavioristic and cognitive versions of the law of effect was originally suggested by problems in teaching new and challenging patterns of behavior to free-living subjects such as children and chimpanzees, which we briefly describe in closing. 3. Tactical Deception in Primates A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne Psychological Laboratories University of St. Andrews, Scotland Tactical deception occurs when an individual's is able to use an "honest" act from his normal repertoire in a different context to mislead familiar individuals. Although primates have a reputation for social skill, most primate groups are so intimate that any deception is likely to be subtle and infrequent. Records are often anecdotal and not widely known in the formal literature of behavioral science. We have tackled this problem by drawing together records from many primates and primatologists in order to look for repeating patterns. This has revealed a many forms of deceptive tactics, which we classify in terms of the function they perform. For each class, we sketch the features of another individual's state of mind that a deceiver must be able to represent, acting as a "natural psychologist." Our analysis clarifies and perhaps explains certain taxonomic differences. Before these findings can be generalized, however, behavioral scientists must agree on some fundamental methodological and theoretical questions in the study of the evolution of social cognition. 4. Event-Related Potentials and Memory: A Critique of the Context Updating Hypothesis Rolf Verleger Mannheim, West Germany P3 is the most prominent of the electrical potentials of the human electroencephalogram that are sensitive to psychological variables. According to the most influential current hypothesis about its psychological significance [E. Donchin's], the "context updating" hypothesis, P3 reflects the updating of working memory. This hypothesis cannot account for relevant portions of the available evidence and it entails some basic contradictions. A more general formulation of this hypothesis is that P3 reflects the updating of expectancies. This version implies that P3- evoking stimuli are initially unexpected but later become expected. This contradiction cannot be resolved within this formulation. The alternative "context closure" hypothesis retains the concept of "strategic information processing" emphasized by the context updating hypothesis. P3s are evoked by events that are awaited when subjects deal with repetitive, highly structured tasks; P3s arise from subjects' combining successive stimuli into larger units The tasks in which P3s are elicited can accordingly be classified in terms of their respective formal sequences of stimuli. P3 may be a physiological indicator of excess activation being released from perceptual control areas. 5. Is the P300 component a manifestation of Context Updating? Emanuel Donchin and Michael G. H. Coles Cognitive Psychophysiology Laboratory University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign [article-length precommentary on Verleger] To understand the endogenous components of the ERP we must use from data about the components' antecedent conditions to form hypotheses about the information processing function of the underlying brain activity. These hypotheses, in turn, generate testable predictions about the consequences of the component. We review the application of this approach to the analysis of the P300 component, whose amplitude is controlled multiplicatively by the subjective probability and the task relevance of the eliciting events and whose latency depends on the duration of stimulus evaluation. These and other factors suggest that the P300 is a manifestation of activity occurring whenever one's model of the environment must be revised. Tests of three predictions based on this "context updating" model are reviewed. Verleger's critique is based on a misconstrual of the model as well as on a partial and misleading reading of the relevant literature. 6. Real and Depicted Spaces: A Cross-Cultural Perspective J.B. Deregowski Psychology Department University of Aberdeen, Scotland This paper examines the contribution of cross-cultural studies to our understanding of the perception and representation of space. A cross-cultural survey of the basic difficulties in understanding pictures -- from the failure to recognize a picture as a representation to the inability to recognise the object represented -- indicates that similar difficulties occur in pictorial and nonpictorial cultures. Real and pictorial spaces must be distinguished. The experimental work on pictorial space derives from two distinct traditions: the study of picture perception in "remote" populations and the study of perceptual illusions. A comparison of the findings on pictorial space perception with those on real space perception and perceptual constancies suggests that cross- cultural differences in the perception of both real and depicted space involve two different kinds of skills: those related only to real spaces or only to depicted spaces and those related to both. Different cultural groups use different skills to perform the same perceptual task. 7. Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework A.W. Logue Department of Psychology SUNY - Stony Brook The tendency to choose a larger, more delayed reinforcer over a smaller, less delayed one (self-control) depends on the current physical values of the reinforcers. It also varies according to a subject's experience and current factors other than the reinforcers. Two local delay models (Mischel's social learning theory and Herrnstein's matching law) as well as molar maximization models have taken into account these indirect effects on self control by representing a subject's behavior as a function of a perceived environment. A general evolutionary analysis of all this research yields a better and more predictive description of self control. ----- This is an experiment in using the Net to find eligible commentators for articles in Behavioral & Brain Sciences. BBS publishes important and controversial interdisciplinary articles in psychology, neuroscience, behavioral biology, cognitive science, artificial intelligence, linguistics and philosophy. Articles are rigorously refereed and, if accepted, are circulated to a large number of potential commentators around the world in the various specialties on which the article impinges. Their 1000-word commentaries are then co-published with the target article as well as the author's response to each. The commentaries consist of analyses, elaborations, complementary and supplementary data and theory, criticisms and cross-specialty syntheses. Commentators are selected by the following means: (1) BBS maintains a computerized file of over 3000 BBS Associates; the size of this group is increased annually as authors, referees, commentators and nominees of current Associates become eligible to become Associates. Many commentators are selected from this list. (2) The BBS editorial office does informal as well as formal computerized literature searches on the topic of the target articles to find additional potential commentators from across specialties and around the world who are not yet BBS Associates. (3) The referees recommend potential commentators. (4) The author recommends potential commentators. We now propose to add the following source for selecting potential commentators: The abstract of the target article will be posted in the relevant newsgroups on the Net. Eligible individuals who judge that they would have a relevant commentary to contribute should contact me at the e-mail address indicated at the bottom of this message, or should write by normal mail to: Stevan Harnad, Editor, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20 Nassau Street, Room 240 Princeton NJ 08542 (phone: 609-921-7771) "Eligibility" usually means being an academically trained professional contributor to one of the disciplines mentioned earlier, or to related academic disciplines. The letter should indicate the candidate's general qualifications as well as their basis for wishing to serve as commentator for the particular target article in question. It is preferable also to enclose a Curriculum Vitae. (Please note that the editorial office must exercise selectivity among the nominations received so as to ensure a strong and balanced cross-specialty spectrum of eligible commentators.) This self-nomination format may also be used by those who wish to become BBS Associates, but they must also specify a current Associate who knows their work and is prepared to nominate them; where no current Associate is known by the candidate, the editorial office will send the Vita to approporiate Associates to ask whether they would be prepared to nominate the candidate. BBS has rapidly become a widely read and highly influential forum in the biobehavioral and cognitive sciences. A recent recalculation of BBS's "impact factor" (ratio of citations to number of articles) in the American Psychologist [41(3) 1986] reports that already in its fifth year of publication (1982) BBS's impact factor had risen to become the highest of all psychology journals indexed as well as 3rd highest of all 1300 journals indexed in the Social Sciences Citation Index and 50th of all 3900 journals indexed in the Science Citation index, which indexes all the scientific disciplines. Potential commentators should send their names, addresses, a description of their general qualifications and their basis for seeking to comment on this target article in particular to the address indicated earlier or to the following e-mail address: harnad@mind.princeton.edu -- Stevan Harnad harnad@mind.princeton.edu (609)4 H0 B229t