[comp.graphics] Frequency distributions of red, green, and blue.

greg@endor.harvard.edu (Greg) (11/14/86)

As most of you know, the color "red" on a monitor is really some distribution
of frequencies of visible light; i.e. when a red piece of phosphorous emits
some amount of light at a wavelength of 2200 angstroms, some different amount
at 2201 angstroms, and so on.  Similarly, a red patch on color film reflects
some known percentage of the incident light at a given frequency.  Lastly, the
red receptors in my eyes respond differently to different wavelengths.  For
example, I may see half as much red if my eye receives 1 unit of light at
wavelength 2000 angstroms as I would see if my eye received 1 unit of light at
2200 angstroms.

Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles of
red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?  I realize
that the characteristics of a color screen and film may vary with the brand, so
I'll mention that I'm using a Sun-3 and Kodachrome film (ASA 100 or slower; I
haven't bought the film yet).  The film will probably be developed on Kodak
paper.
----
Greg

spf@bonnie.ATT.COM (11/14/86)

From clyde!rutgers!seismo!husc6!endor!greg Fri Nov 14 09:58:22 EST 1986
Organization: Harvard

>Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles of
>red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
>film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?
>Greg
>
You've fallen into a trap common to computer technologists and
engineers: you've assumed that the eye and brain work like a computer.
The cones in the eye's retina respond differentially to frequency
of the incoming radiation.  There are not three well-defined types
of receptors which one would call red, green, and blue.  Indeed, 
red, green, and blue refer to subjective color judgments, not
absolutes.  For a given individual you can measure the frequency's
which they REPORT as red, green and blue, but they will be different
for different folks.
Enjoy!
Steve

btb@ncoast.UUCP (Brad Banko) (11/16/86)

for questions about light and color, Eastman Kodak is probably the place
to go... I would recommend that you contact them (Rochester, NY ?)...
I understand that they take light and color very seriously (hope this is
useful).
-- 
			Brad Banko
			...!decvax!cwruecmp!ncoast!btb
			Cleveland, Ohio

john@frog.UUCP (John Woods, Software) (11/17/86)

>From clyde!rutgers!seismo!husc6!endor!greg Fri Nov 14 09:58:22 EST 1986
>>Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles
>>of red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
>>film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?
>>Greg
> >
> You've fallen into a trap common to computer technologists and
> engineers: you've assumed that the eye and brain work like a computer.
> The cones in the eye's retina respond differentially to frequency
> of the incoming radiation.

First, the cones individually do respond to particular bands of light
preferentially (see articles in the past few months in SCIENCE and Scientific
American), roughly centering on "red", "blue", and "green".

However, perceiving color IS done differentially, and in Freshman Physics at
MIT, we learned about this (I believe it was discovered by Edwin Land), and
were given a demonstration:  a black and white picture of a clown with
brightly colored candy, taken with a blue filter, and the same picture taken
without a filter (and, due to the characteristics of the film, having the
strongest color sensitivity in the yellow area spectrum).  When these two
images were projected onto the screen, we saw full color (reds, greens, you
name it), even though these colors weren't in either slide.  It was stated
(without demonstration) that you can get the same effect by making photos
using the two yellow lines of sodium -- that tiny difference in frequency
allows the brain to reconstruct the color of the actual image!

--
John Woods, Charles River Data Systems, Framingham MA, (617) 626-1101
...!decvax!frog!john, ...!mit-eddie!jfw, jfw%mit-ccc@MIT-XX.ARPA

"Soylent Green is People Helping People!"

dan@rna.UUCP (Dan Ts'o) (11/20/86)

>From clyde!rutgers!seismo!husc6!endor!greg Fri Nov 14 09:58:22 EST 1986
>Organization: Harvard
>
>>Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles of
>>red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
>>film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?
>>Greg
>>
>You've fallen into a trap common to computer technologists and
>engineers: you've assumed that the eye and brain work like a computer.
>The cones in the eye's retina respond differentially to frequency
>of the incoming radiation.  There are not three well-defined types
>of receptors which one would call red, green, and blue.

	Hmm... A bit of disinformation here...

	There, of course, ARE three well-define cone receptors as well as the
rods. The spectral sensitivity of the cones DO peak in the red, green and blue.
However, they are broadly tuned such that, e.g., a monochromatic green light
will also excite (to a lesser extent) the red cones. This is as it should be,
since three sharply tuned receptors would show nothing for light outside their
bands, forcing the necessity for many more than three receptors.

	Yes, the beginnings of color vision is computed early on differentially.
The concept is called color opponency. There are two major color opponent
systems known: red/green and blue/yellow. There may also be a green/blue
opponeny system. Yellow is thought to be derived from the summed inputs from
red and green cones.

	However, color perception is much more complicated than that. One
important property which indicates this is called color constancy. Color
constancy is a high level computation performed somewhat more globally (in
visual space.) It is manifest, e.g. in the relative invariance of the perceived
color of a group of object, irrespective of the spectral content of the
illumination. For example, things seem to have nearly the same color under a
wide range of illuminations: dawn, dusk, midday, even under highly colored
lighting. The point is, color perception is much more complicated than just
detecting the wavelengths of light arriving at the retina.

	Back to the original question. The exact output of a color monitor
is, of course, dependent on the phosphors that a monitor uses, among other
things. There are quite a few phosphors available. The bottom line is that
you need to find out what monitor you have and what options are on it (the
most often distinction is between "standard" and long persistence phosphors)
and then contact the manufacturer for the curves. Those companies well-based
in the U.S. are usually more capable of answering the question. For example,
I have the output curves for my Tektronics 690SR and a Conrac monitor. (If
you're just curious, I could perhaps describe them to you by email.)

	As far as the spectral sensitivity of the human cones, there are
numerous references and methodologies. The most classical studies just
measured via microspectroscopy, the absorption curves of the cone pigments.
Then there are psychophysical and physiological measurements. Most of the
results from these methods coinside, though not completely.

	Perhaps the best place to start is a standard reference text on the
retina (any decent science or medical library should have one). An example
is, I believe, Retina by Rodieck.

	BTW, since when were computers able to "see" ? :-)

smolar@winston.UUCP (Stephan Smolar) (11/22/86)

Tests were done on the human eye for standards to be set for
color television phosphors bye the CIE (France) and the ICI
(US) International Commission of Illumination. The CIE is
recommended as standards by the Standards committee of the
Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers.

Results.

From the Wald and Brown (1965) experiments the three peak
sensitivies of the cones (color sensors are 450, 530 and 570
nanometers.

The CIE primaries are 
Red:    700 nm
Green:  546.1 nm
Blue:   435.8 nm
  
The FCC phosphor Wavelengths are

Red:	610 nm
Green:	535 nm
Blue:	460 nm

I believe that each film in cameras had different pigments
and there is no standard set in stone. However of that I am
not sure.

Ref: Television Broadcasting by Harold E. Ennes 
     The Psychology of Visual Perception by R. Haber and M.
     Hershenson

I hope this is of some help.
-- 

Stephan Smolar			| ..decvax!microsoft!ubc-vision!winston!smolar
New Media Technologies Ltd.	| ..ihnp4!alberta!ubc-vision!winston!smolar
(604) 291-7111			|

kgd@rlvd.UUCP (11/22/86)

In article <647@husc6.HARVARD.EDU> greg@endor.UUCP (Greg) writes:
>
>Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles of
>red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
>film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?  I realize
>that the characteristics of a color screen and film may vary with the brand so
>I'll mention that I'm using a Sun-3 and Kodachrome film (ASA 100 or slower; I
>haven't bought the film yet).  The film will probably be developed on Kodak
>paper.
 
This problem is further complicated by the fact that the sensitivity of
the three types of photopigment to wave-lengths of light in the human
eye varies between individuals and with age.
 
The best explanation I have seen was in a tutorial paper by Gerald Murch.
His address is given as:  Tektronix Inc, PO Box 500, Beaverton, Oregon
97077, USA.
 
-- 
Keith Dancey,                                UUCP:   ..!mcvax!ukc!rlvd!kgd
Rutherford Appleton Laboratory,
Chilton, Didcot, Oxon  OX11 0QX             
                                            JANET:       K.DANCEY@uk.ac.rl
Tel: (0235) 21900   ext 5716

jbm@aurora.UUCP (Jeffrey Mulligan) (11/22/86)

> 
> Does anyone have know (or have references to) the exact frequency profiles of
> red, green, and blue phosphorous on a color monitor, red, green, and blue
> film, and the red, green, and blue receptors in a person's eye?  I realize
> that the characteristics of a color screen and film may vary with the brand, so

The book "Color Science" by Wyszecki and Stiles is pretty much the
"bible" of color vision; it contains, among other things, the action
spectra of the three cone mechanisms.  For many purposes, it is
more convenient to describe a color by its effect on the cones rather
than by the complete spectrum, since many different spectral distributions
will be visually indistinguishable ("metameric matches").  The
most commonly used system is CIE (a French acronym for the society
that maintains the standard).  Some manufacturers (such as Tektronix)
give the CIE coordinates of the various phosphor options along
with the other monitor specs; unfortunately, this is not true of many
other monitors.  To make matters worse, knowing the phosphor chromaticities
does not tell you what you will get when you drive the red GUN,
unless the purity adjustment is *perfect*.

Jeff Mulligan