ee173xed@sdcc3.UUCP ({|stu) (12/02/84)
Until comparatively recently Western music theorists and the textbooks written for students of music theory have concerned themselves almost exclusively with the interac- tions of pitches. The study of harmony, chord progressions, and melody, while very important to the structure of tradi- tional music, neglects an important element of sound. Any- one who listens to music can hear the difference between a melody played by a flute and that played by a guitar or vio- lin. Yet it has not been easy to articulate the details of distinction, nor has the contrast always seemed to be impor- tant to the composer. The music that has survived from Medieval and Renais- sance Europe rarely indicates the instruments for which the music was intended. The theoretical treatises of the time, particularly Praetorius' _S_y_n_t_a_g_m_a _M_u_s_i_c_u_m (16th century), give detailed descriptions and drawings of many of the ancient instruments, and historical interest has led to the reconstruction of many of them. From these and the few specimens that have survived as museum pieces, whoever wishes can hear what early music might have sounded like. Many musicologists believe that performances in the Renaissance usually consisted of whatever instrumentalists were available, and certainly vocal music predominated, if only by virtue of the greater number of singers. In fact, most polyphonic lines were written in a vocal style and could be either sung or played. Not until the early Baroque era did composers begin to specify instrumentations in their scores. And not until the late Classic period, especially with Beethoven, was music written that was truly dependent upon the differences between instruments. Beethoven and composers ever since the end of the Classic period have included in their orchetsral writing specific instrucions as to the number and type of instruments that were to be sound- ing, as well as the dynamic level of each instrument. Pas- sages of important melodic ideas would not be heard, nor would the intended musical effect be achieved if the dynam- ics were not observed or if the instrumentation were changed. In effect, composers required control of the color of the orchestra. The word most commonly used by speakers of modern English to denote the difference in the quality of sound between instruments is defined concisely, but somewhat vaguely, in its standard dictionary version (Webster): "Timbre - the characteristic quality of a sound that distin- guishes one voice or musical instrument from another: it is determined by the harmonics of the sound and is dis- tinguished from the intensity and pitch." Alternatives to the word "timbre" (which means "postage stamp" in France and means "doorbell" in Latin America and Spain) have been offered, including color, tone quality, and Klangfarbe (from the German). A direct translation of Klangfarbe has proved unworkable, as Alexander Ellis, the translator of Helmholtz's _O_n _t_h_e _S_e_n_s_a_t_i_o_n _o_f _T_o_n_e (1877), humorously sum- marized: Prof. Helmholtz uses the word Klang for a musical tone, which generally, but not always, means a com- pound tone. Prof. Tyndall therefore proposes to use the English word clang in the same sense. . . Of course, if clang could not be used, Prof. Tyndall's suggestion to translate Prof. Helmholtz's Klangfarbe by clangtint fell to the ground. I can find no valid reason for supplanting the time-honored expression quality of tone. Prof. Tyndall quotes Dr. Young to the effect that "this quality of sound is sometimes called its register, colour, or tim- bre." Register has a distinct meaning in vocal music which must not be disturbed. Timbre . . . is a foreign word, often odiously mispronounced, and not worth preserving. "Clangtint" does sound a bit strange. However, Ellis lost his fight to discredit the use of timbre, and current text- books in physics and acoustics are specific, although not complete enough for some, in their definition of it: "The subjective measure of the number and relative strengths of the overtones present, in addition to the fundamental. It is represented as the shape of the wave, its waveform." (Askill, 1979) The idea of timbre being a function of the relative strengths of the overtones in the complex waveform can be credited principally to the work of the nineteenth-century physicists and mathematicians Fourier, Ohm, and Helmholtz. - 4 - Fourier states that every complex tone is made up of a number of sinusoidal tones of different frequencies and intensities, each sinusoid having a frequency that is an integral multiple or harmonic of the lowest or fundamental frequency. Stated simply, every musical tone is made up of a fundamental sine wave accompanied by other sine waves which are called overtones, harmonics, or partials, depending on the context. Ohm maintains that each of the separate fre- quencies in the complex sound is audible and can be per- ceived separately. Composers such as Stockhausen in this century have shown, by use of the electronic medium, that pitch, rhythm, and timbre are related. If a tone or a rhythm is repeated quickly enough, so that the rate of the repetition falls into the audible range (above about 20 repetitions per second), that series of repetitions will be heard as a sin- gle tone with its own entirely different timbre. Conversely, if the pitch of a complex tone is steadily lowered, until the fundamental is no longer audible, it will be perceived as a periodic rhythm containing distinct pitches and tim- bres. In a similar vein, present-day composers have written music that is dependent primarily upon timbre as a unifying compositional principle. Such pieces are often called Klangfarbenmelodies, and are illustrations of what can be done if timbre, instead of harmony, is paramount. Helmholtz (1877), building on the theory of Ohm, - 5 - developed what is called the Place Theory of pitch percep- tion. This theory describes the cochlea of the inner ear as a natural frequency decoder. Each of the 25 to 30 thousand hair fibers along the basilar membrane resonates at a dif- ferent frequency, the fibers near to the oval window where the basilar membrane is thinnest corresponding to the higher frequencies, and those fibers at the end of the basilar mem- brane where it is thickest corresponding to the lower fre- quencies. A complex waveform excites the fibers of the basilar membrane in a one-to-one relationship with the over- tones present. The relative strength of each of the over- tones is expressed as a measure of the intensity of the resonance of the hair fibers involved. This information is then transmitted to the brain by the nervous system, where the information is assimilated and interpreted. The timbre of a sound in the Place Theory model is perceived directly from the component sinusoidal frequencies that make up the complex waveform. Much of the present-day work in acoustics follows the model given by Helmholtz. Plomp (1976) believes that the ear performs a frequency analysis of the complex waveform, but that the listener may not be aware of the existence of the individual harmonics. Rather, the harmonics fuse into a single percept. Plomp acknowledges the importance of the change over time of the relative strengths of the individual harmonics in the complex tone, but discusses only the steady state and the dimensions present in the Fourier theorem. - 6 - The following equation demonstrates the multidimensionality of the complex tone: w(t) = a(k) sin(2 pi k f t + p(k)) where w(t) represents the complex waveform as a summation of the sinusoidal components k as the wave develops through time t, for k = 1 to the highest harmonic having any ampli- tude. The variables, a - amplitude of the harmonic k, f - fundamental frequency, and p - phase of the component k, form the shape and timbre of the waveform. It should be quite apparent that the phase of the indi- vidual partials radically alters the shape of the waveform, since the waveshape is a summation of the separate sine waves that account for each partial. The perceived timbre, on the other hand, is affected by phase, but not to a great degree. Immensely differing waveshapes, distinct in their spectral makeup only in the phase of the sinusoidal com- ponents, sound very similar. Generally, the effect of vary- ing the phase angle is noticeable only for the higher har- monics of a complex tone. Actual complex timbres, even in the steady state, do not usually have overtones that are exact integer multiples of the fundamental. For example, analysis has shown that the harmonics of a bowed string closely approach the ideal, but the harmonics of a plucked string do not form integral - 7 - ratios to the fundamental, as anyone who tries to tune a guitar by using the harmonics will discover. Musicians are generally not content to believe that the steady state of a tone's spectrum is representative of its timbre. The change of the spectral and amplitude envelopes through the duration of the note, usually called the attack, the initial decay, the sustain level, and the release, are of great importance to the development of timbre. The spec- trum and amplitude of a tone vary markedly, especially dur- ing the attack and final release portions of its duration. An interesting experiment (Grey and Moorer, 1977) involves the analysis and resynthesis of the timbres of various musical instruments using the additive Fourier model, with the frequencies of the overtones and fundamental as well as the amplitudes of the harmonics being time vari- ant. To simplify the procedure, the tones chosen for analysis purposely lack vibrato. The minute variations of amplitude and frequency in the analyzed spectrum are replaced in the resynthesis with straight line-segment approximations. The resulting synthetic timbre is, in the estimation of the researchers, virtually the same as the original to the human perceiver, even though definite changes in the waveform have been made. When composers began writing for specific ensembles in the Baroque era, they also began to include such timbre - 8 - notations in their scores as phrasing, legato or stacatto tongueing or bowing, and dynamic markings. Between that time and the present day the notation of attacks, releases, phrasing, color combinations and transformations, and dynam- ics has become more and more specific. Detailed instrucions to a violin performer such as "am Steg," "col legno," and "stop the A string with the left thumb at A# and with the fourth finger of the left hand glissando from the octave to the highest possible harmonic while bowing gently," have become popular with modern composers. Harmonic analysis has helped to categorize the differ- ences between instruments. The flute, for example, produces a tone with few strong overtones, most closely approaching a pure sine wave of all the orchestral instruments. The clar- inet, in contrast, has a prominent fundamental but also has strong odd-numbered partials. The oboe and the bassoon exhibit what are often called formant regions--regions in the range of the instrument that are resonant and cause par- tials that fall there to be much higher in amplitude. For the oboe, the formant regions are from 1000-1500 hertz and again at 3000-4000 hertz. For this reason, low notes on the oboe (or bassoon) lack energy at the fundamental and appear to be higher than the register in which they are notated. Brass instrments are rich in partials in all but the highest part of the range. A formant region covers approximately the upper two-thirds of the range, causing the lower notes to have weak fundamentals. Also, the louder a note is played - 9 - on a brass instrument, the stronger the upper partials become. The theories discussed so far do not adequately explain how the mind is able to differentiate between the various timbres present or how they are related to those timbres previously encountered by an individual perceiver. One popular theory of cognitive perception is the general notion of "template matching" (Norman/Lindsay, 1977). It is pro- posed that a record is kept and categorized by the brain in the form of a template for each stimulus encountered by the individual. When the brain is presented with a new stimulus, the existing templates are searched for a match. If a partial or near match can be found, the stimulus is then identified and categorized, else a new category is formed. In the case of timbre, or any other multidimensional stimulus, a host of parameters must be stored somewhere in memory for retrieval in the template matching strategy. Consider the clarinet, whose tone color is noticeably dif- ferent in the low chalumeau register from its timbre in the higher range. A listener will correctly identify all sounds produced by the instrument as being typical of a clarinet, yet a separate spectral template for each sound would have to be stored in memory in order to make the positive iden- tification possible. Within the template must be placed all the spectral components obtained from the Place Theory - 10 - additive analysis of timbre. Clearly, the storage capacity required of the mind by this process is enormous. Other cognitive viewpoints exist and are sometimes at variance with one another. For example, the Periodicity Theory of pitch perception, in contrast to the Place Theory, attributes an ability to the basilar membrane of being able to decode frequency as a function of the resonance of the membrane, where the nerve cells react to the velocity pat- terns of the airwave and fire in synchrony with the regular rise and fall of the beat frequency. However, with either the Place or Periodicity theories the Fourier model is left intact, the ear performing the spectral analysis and sending the information to the brain for template matching. A favor- ite technique, and a major problem with the development of cognitive theories, is the use of illusions or other methods of fooling the perceptual system in an effort to understand and explain the mechanism. As interesting as illusions may be, when undue emphasis is placed on their study, all they really begin to show is how the perceptual system may be fooled, not how it works. The finding in another experiment by Grey (1975) involves the relationship between timbres of various musical instruments. Two instrument timbres are chosen, analyzed, and resynthesized according to the additive technique already described. An interpolation of the time-variant amplitude/frequency spectra is made in several successive - 11 - steps between the two instrument timbres. The observers are asked to determine the point at which the timbre of the second instrument becomes noticeable, and to note the abruptness or gradualness of the transition. It is interesting that there are no naturally-occuring instrument sounds between discreet musical instruments. Rather, the range between the two instruments exhibits properties of both timbres, more like a blend than a separate third tim- bre. Also of note is that the moment chosen by the observers as the point of crossover from one instrument to the next invariably is closer to the second instrument than to the first, exibiting a definite hysteresis in all transi- tions. Sound clearly has many dimensions including loudness, intensity, pitch, and timbre. Many of these attibutes them- selves are multidimensional. Pitch, for example, has at least the two dimensions of pitch-class (C, C#, D, D# . . .) and octave. An investigation into the facets of timbre benefits from a device called multidimensional scaling. In this system, perceptual data consisting of subjective simi- larity judgements between pairs in a set of stimuli are treated as measurements of subjective distance from which a best-fitting geometric image, with the number of dimensions specified by the investigator, is constructed. In one such experiment (Grey, 1977), sixteen instrument tones were pro- cessed according to the analysis/synthesis routine and equalized for loudness, pitch, and duration, in order to - 12 - remove those dimensions from the analysis. It was found that three spatial dimensions best represent the perceptual relationships: (1) spectral energy distribution (wide band versus narrow), (2) the presence of low-amplitude high- frequency enery in the attack segment (most often inhar- monic) as opposed to low frequency only, and (3) the degree of synchronicity of the amplitudes of the harmonics in their temporal progressions. As this was a test using specific soundtypes, other dimensions are also possible in a dif- ferent setting. Using the time-variant amplitude synthesis technique to represent various instruments involves the manipulation of a large amount of data, and requires a substantial amount of computational power (ie., a fast digital computer). Yet it is plain that not all the variables or dimensions have been taken into account. Each new dimension adds more complexity to the information needed to be stored if the template theory is to be satisfactorily implemented. The increasing complexity as new parameters are identified and added to the model suggests that there should be a simpler, more general way of approaching timbre. Harmonic analysis, using the Fourier theory, yields a list of the frequencies present and their relative ampli- tudes over a given time segment or window. If an accurate picture of a timbre is desired which includes the continuous - 13 - time-variant information, many thousands of Fourier transforms of sufficiently small window size are required. To suggest that the ear works in this way, that it uses the Fourier transform to obtain its harmonic data, is untenable because of the impossibility of knowing where and when to perform the Fourier analyses. However, the ear is able to detect information specific to the dynamic events that pro- duced the timbre, the sound source. Musical instruments and all natural sound sources can be modeled as dynamic systems, each with its own style of change over time. The source would then be taken as an independent solution, with its own set of boundary conditions and variables, to a wave equa- tion, however complex it may need to be, such as that described by Hiller and Ruiz (1971) for various plucked, struck, and bowed strings. The method by which an object or air column is excited into vibration is specific to that system. The attack transients, steady state, release tran- sients, the presence or absence of vibrato (controlled vari- ations in frequency) or tremelo (variations in amplitude), and the characteristic transients when moving between notes--all of the perceptible timbre cues--are functions of the source of the sound. Gibson (1966) describes an alternative approach to the cognitive model of visual perception which he calls ecologi- cal optics. Ecological acoustics, after Gibson's model, advocates an approach to the perception of sound that com- bines the physical analysis of the source event and the - 14 - identification of the higher-order acoustic properties of the event that are detectable by the listener. An analogy can be drawn between the action of human perception mechan- isms and operation of an interesting device called a polar planimeter (Runeson, 1977). This device, while adept at measuring the area of an irregular outline or shape, is totally unsuited to the task of measuring the length of an object, even though observers would remark that it should be impossible to calculate area without being able to measure length since length is part of the complex quantity--area. If the senses can be modeled as direct receptors of complex variables, then it becomes easier to believe that the ear can detect timbre without resorting to the Fourier transform. In fact, the human perceptual system ignores cer- tain variables in the environment that other species con- sider important--for example, the frequencies over about 20,000 hertz that dogs find useful, or the pattern of skylight polarization that only insects can see. In support of the Ecological view, Heyser (1976) suggests the thesis that ". . . we reaize that a frequency domain expansion can be completely accurate and yet have no meaning to a . . . listener because it is in the wrong system of coordinates," because the frequency domain and the time domain are really just two different ways of looking at the same thing, and the ear is only perceptive of the latter. Balzano (1983) maintains that if the basic form of the underlying dynamics of a sound stimulus are invariant over its intensity range - 15 - and its frequency range, and if the ear can perceive infor- mation of this sort directly, then the subconcious spectral analyses and the encyclopedic memory for template matching are not needed in the identification of timbres. With the understanding that the method of tone generation is central to the identification of timbre, it is easy to see why many people group all of the synthesized sound that has been pro- duced this century into a single "timbre-electronic." The development of a workable definition of timbre has been dominated for many years by the Helmholtz spectral theory. Acousticians working more than a hundred years after the proposal of Helmholtz have only expanded the original theory, and are still primarily concerned with the steady state timbre. Musicians recognize the importance of the time-varying spectrum, and researchers can demonstrate com- petent examples of analysis and resynthesis using multidi- mensional time-variant parameters. Cognitive perceptualists accept the Helmholtz Place Theory but offer the contrasting Periodicity Theory, yet with either theory they rely on the unwieldy system of template matching for information recog- nition and retrieval. Ecological acousticians are interested in the development of an alternative view to the Helmholtz/Fourier model, suggesting that timbre and the per- ception of timbre, instead of being the performance of abstract analyses of the perceived sound structure, are a matter of directly perceiving and interpreting the underly- ing dynamics of the physical process that generates the - 16 - sound. - 17 - _B_i_b_l_i_o_g_r_a_p_h_y [1] Balzano, Gerald J. "Changing Conceptions of Pitch and Timbre: A Modest Proposal." Paper presented to the 106th meeting of the Acoustical Society of Amer- ica. Abstract in supplement to vol. 74 (November 1983): 518. [2] ________. "Musical vs. Psychoacoustical Variables and Their Influence on the Perception of Musical Inter- vals." _B_u_l_l_e_t_i_n _o_f _t_h_e _C_o_u_n_c_i_l _f_o_r _R_e_a_s_e_a_r_c_h _i_n _M_u_s_i_c _E_d_u_c_a_t_i_o_n 70 (1982): 1-11. [3] Boretz, B. and Cone, E. (Eds.) _P_e_r_s_p_e_c_t_i_v_e_s _o_n _C_o_n_t_e_m_p_o_r_a_r_y _M_u_s_i_c _T_h_e_o_r_y. New York: Norton, 1972. Babbitt, M. "The Structure and Function of Musical Theory." Randall, J. K. "Two Lectures to Scientists." [4] Clark, Melville and Luce, David. "Intensities of Orchestral Instrument Scales Played at Prescribed Dynamic Markings." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_u_d_i_o _E_n_g_i_n_e_e_r_i_n_g _S_o_c_i_e_t_y 13, 2 (1965): 151 - 57. [5] Clark, M., Robertson, P., and Luce, D. "A Preliminary Experiment on the Perceptual Basis for Musical Instrument Families." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_u_d_i_o _E_n_g_i_n_e_e_r_i_n_g _S_o_c_i_e_t_y 12 (1964): 199-203. [6] Clark, Melville and Paul Milner. "Dependence of Timbre on the Tonal Loudness Produced by Musical Instruments." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_u_d_i_o _E_n_g_i_n_e_e_r_i_n_g _S_o_c_i_e_t_y 12, 1 (1964): 28 - 31. [7] Cogan, Robert and Pozzi Escot. _S_o_n_i_c _D_e_s_i_g_n. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1976. Chapter 4, "The Color of Sound." - 18 - [8] Erickson, Robert. _S_o_u_n_d _S_t_r_u_c_t_u_r_e _i_n _M_u_s_i_c. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1975. pp. 1-47. [9] Fechner, Gustav. _E_l_e_m_e_n_t_s _o_f _P_s_y_c_h_o_p_h_y_s_i_c_s. Vol. 1. Trans. by Helmut E. Adler. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1965. Preface, chaps. 1-4, 9. [10] Gibson, Eleanor. _P_r_i_n_c_i_p_l_e_s _o_f _P_e_r_c_e_p_t_u_a_l _L_e_a_r_n_i_n_g. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969. Chs. 1-5, 20. [11] Gibson, J. J. _T_h_e _S_e_n_s_e_s _C_o_n_s_i_d_e_r_e_d _a_s _P_e_r_c_e_p_t_u_a_l _S_y_s_t_e_m_s. Boston: Houghton, 1966. Introduction, Chs. 1-3, 5, 13-14. [12] Grey, J. "An Exploration of Musical Timbre." Dissertation. Department of Psycology, Stanford University, 1975. pp. 75 - 86. [13] ________. "Multidimensional Perceptual Scaling of Musical Timbres." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 61, 5 (1977): 1270 - 1277. [14] ________. "Timbre Discrimination in Musical Patterns." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 64, 2 (1978): 467 - 473. [15] ________, and J. W. Gordon. "Perceptual Effects of Spectral Modifications on Musical Timbres." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 63, 5 (1978): 1493 - 1500. [16] ________, and J. A. Moorer. "Perceptual Evaluation of Synthesized Musical Instrument Tones." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 62, 2 (1977): 454 - 462. - 19 - [17] Helmholtz, H. L. F. _D_i_e _L_e_h_r_e _v_o_n _d_e_n _T_o_n_e_m_p_f_i_n_d_u_n_g_e_n. Brunswick: 1863. English trans., _O_n _t_h_e _S_e_n_s_a_t_i_o_n_s _o_f _T_o_n_e. New York: Dover, 1954. Introduction, Chs. 1-4, 19. [18] Hiller, L. and Isaacson. "Experimental Music." In K.M. Sayre, ed., _T_h_e _M_o_d_e_l_i_n_g _o_f _M_i_n_d. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1963. [19] Heyser, R.C. "Perspectives in Audio Analysis: Changing the Frame of Reference." I. _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_u_d_i_o _E_n_g_i_n_e_e_r_i_n_g _S_o_c_i_e_t_y 24, 8 (October 1976), 660 - 667. II. _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_u_d_i_o _E_n_g_i_n_e_e_r_i_n_g _S_o_c_i_e_t_y 24, 9 (November 1976), 742 - 751. [20] Lindsay, P., and Norman, D. A. _H_u_m_a_n _I_n_f_o_r_m_a_t_i_o_n _P_r_o_c_e_s_s_i_n_g. New York, 1972. Chapts. 1, 4, 5, 7. [21] McAdams, Stephen and Albert Bregman. "Hearing Musical Streams." _C_o_m_p_u_t_e_r _M_u_s_i_c _J_o_u_r_n_a_l 3, 4 (December 1979): 26-43, 60, 63. [22] Meyer, Leonard. _E_m_o_t_i_o_n _a_n_d _M_e_a_n_i_n_g _i_n _M_u_s_i_c. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956. Chs. 1-3. [23] Miller, George. "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Information." _P_s_y_c_o_l_o_g_i_c_a_l _R_e_v_i_e_w 63 (1956): 81-97. [24] Moorer, James Anderson. "Music and Computer Composition." _C_o_m_m_u_n_i_c_a_t_i_o_n_s _o_f _t_h_e _A_s_s_o_c_i_a_t_i_o_n _f_o_r _C_o_m_p_u_t_i_n_g _M_a_c_h_i_n_e_r_y (_A_C_M) 15 (1972): 104-113. [25] _____. "On the transcription of musical sound by computer." _C_o_m_p_u_t_e_r _M_u_s_i_c _J_o_u_r_n_a_l 1, 4 (1977): 32-38. - 20 - [26] Patterson, Blake. "Musical Dynamics." _S_c_i_e_n_t_i_f_i_c _A_m_e_r_i_c_a_n 231, 5 (1979): 78 - 95. [27] Pikler, Andrew G. "Mels and Musical Intervals." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _M_u_s_i_c _T_h_e_o_r_y 10 (1966): 288-299. [28] Plomp, R. _A_s_p_e_c_t_s _o_f _T_o_n_e _S_e_n_s_a_t_i_o_n. New York: Academic Press, 1976. Chapts. 1, 6, 7. [29] Randall, J. K. "A Report From Princeton." _P_e_r_s_p_e_c_t_i_v_e_s _o_f _N_e_w _M_u_s_i_c 3, 2 (1964/65): 84 - 92. [30] Revesz, G. _I_n_t_r_o_d_u_c_t_i_o_n _t_o _t_h_e _P_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y _o_f _M_u_s_i_c. Leipzig, 1913. Chapts. 1, 4, 5, 6, 12, 14. [31] Runeson, Sverker. "On the Possibility of `Smart' Perceptual Mechanisms." _S_c_a_n_d_i_n_a_v_i_a_n _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _P_s_y_c_o_l_o_g_y 18 (1977): 172-179. [32] Saldanha, E. and Corso, J. "Timbre Cues for the Recognition of Musical Instruments." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 36 (1964): 2021- 2026. [33] Slawson, Wayne. "The Color of Sound: A Theoretical Study in Musical Timbre." _M_u_s_i_c _T_h_e_o_r_y _S_p_e_c_t_r_u_m 3 (1981): 132 - 141. [34] Strong, W. and Clark, M. "Synthesis of Wind-Instrument Tones." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_- _i_c_a 41 (1967): 39-52. [35]_____. "Perturbations of Synthetic Orchestral Wind- Instrument Tones." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _t_h_e _A_c_o_u_s_t_i_c_a_l _S_o_c_i_e_t_y _o_f _A_m_e_r_i_c_a 41 (1967): 277-285. - 21 - [36] Tenney, James C. "The Physical Correlates of Timbre." _G_r_a_v_e_s_a_n_e_r _B_l_a_t_t_n_e_r 26 (1965): 106 - 109. [37] Warner, Daniel. "Notes from the Timbre Space." _P_e_r_s_p_e_c_t_i_v_e_s _o_f _N_e_w _M_u_s_i_c 21 (1982): 15-22. [38] Warren, William H., and Robert R. Verbrugge. "Auditory Perception of Breaking and Bouncing Events: A Case Study in Ecological Acoustics." _J_o_u_r_n_a_l _o_f _E_x_p_e_r_i_- _m_e_n_t_a_l _P_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y: _H_u_m_a_n _P_e_r_c_e_p_t_i_o_n _a_n_d _P_e_r_f_o_r_m_a_n_c_e 10, 5 (1984): 704 - 712.