man@bocar.UUCP (M Nevar) (05/14/85)
This is an editorial that appeared in Stereophile Magazine Volume 6 Number 6 that came out in the Spring (?) of 1984. It is reprinted by request of you netters. It is long. If you have any interest in high-end or speaker cables, read it, it is VERY interesting. Well, here goes: The Horse's Mouth It's not often that you get a chance to have extensive discussions with the horse's mouth, but we recently had that opportunity. Since July of this year (1984) there has been extensive discussion in the audio community, particularly the high-end segment, of Larry Greenhill's review of three speaker cables in Stereo Review (August, 1983, Ziff-Davis). From recent talks with author Greenhill we've learned that that the most interesting story was *not* in Stereo Review: instead it can be found in the varying reactions from different quarters, and what they say about the high-end industry in general. The commentaries have been many and varied. About six weeks after the SR article appeared, we were privileged to receive the preprint of a critique by Peter Moncrieff, of IAR. This critique consumed 14 pages and was unusually circumlocutory, even for Peter. It went to great lengths to pound SR's point of view into the earth, along with it, a piece of the readers patience. Many just criticisms of Greenhill's article (as published in Stereo Review) were made, but that's not all; IAR's critique went on to introduce questionable theoretical constructs: a concept known as "the vinegar effect"; an analogy which likened the performance anxieties surely experienced by the listening panel to sexual performance anxieties; a radical proposition with respect to left-brain right-brain separation as it affects the outcome of psychoacoustic experiments. Unfortunately, in my eyes, the IAR critique's ponderous length and doubtful points obscured what was in fact legitimate criticism of the SR cable review. IAR circulated this preprint to a variety of high-end manufacturers. The purpose was to alert this community to the danger posed by the variety of misleading analyses employed in SR's speaker cable article, to decry Stereo Review's standard position on the audible differences (the title of the critique is "Three Strikes and You're Out"), and to suggest a boycott of Stereo Review's advertising pages. Responses were solicited with the promise of printing them intact in whatever form they came. To my mind, the most remarkable aspect of the IAR story was its consumption of an entire issue of IAR Hotline. I could only think, "Gee, Peter, why spend so much time demonstrating what everyone's known for time immemorial: Stereo Review is committed to badmouthing high-end audio." I was surprised when IAR Hotline eventually came out; a large number of people had actually written to voice dismay at the speaker cable article, to testify as to the audibility of different speaker cables, and to decry high- end's lack of muscle in the marketplace. Apparently the issue m=was more controversial than I thought. Then I heard of even more reaction. Hans Fantel of The New York Times wrote a piece pointing to the Stereo Review article as yet more proof that high-end audio people were raving lunatics in their insistence that seemingly minor factors can have major effects on the sound. Gregory Sandow of The Absolute Sound wrote a piece for The Wall Street Journal (Nov 29, 1983) pointing up the differences between TAS (and its ilk) and Stereo Review as exemplified by Larry Greenhill's article on speaker cables. In their September issue, TAS ran a comment on the SR article, a few letters in response, and a very funny "play" starring Julian Hearse, Antonio Stradivarius (sic), David Ramada-Inn, Gordon Sales, and Hairy Person. By this time Stereophile readers who haven't read SR's original article should be running to their shelves, garbages, or libraries to see what possibly could have been said that would generate all this controversy. And that's just the point: controversy, no matter the source, generates interest and sells magazines. The most interesting comment of all comes from William Livingston, editor of Stereo Review, in a letter circulated to SR's "comp" list and addressed to "Friends of Stereo Review": "In the August (1983) issue of Stereo Review we published an article by Laurence Greenhill in which he described the results of listening tests... The article turned out to be a somewhat bigger news story than we realized... The letters came from everywhere from Ogden, Utah, to Montevideo, Uruguay... I have never responded to the charge that Stereo Review is an enemy of the high end. Anyone who reads the magazine knows that this charge is unfounded... While such letters may not prove anything conclusive about speaker cables, they prove to me that Stereo Review is a magazine with a very high degree of reader involvement. We are a magazine that makes a difference." Not surprisingly from the tone of this letter, SR's "comp" list is composed mostly of advertisers. What does Stereophile think of the SR cable review flap? Well, we're in an unusual position, because we've seen both the article Stereo Review printed *and* the original article as submitted by Larry Greenhill. I spent some of my undergraduate years studying the New Testament. Much of the New Testament scholarship is devoted to analyzing the different contributions made by the "authors" of the different gospels as they passed along orally the traditions they had received. believe me, if the evangelists made the changes in their oral traditions that Stereo Review made to Larry Greenhill's article, the New Testament might read like Fear of Flying. Granted, SR reported the data just as Larry gathered it -- in fact, some of the data actually contradicts the written part of the article. then, of course, SR inserted a lot of their editorial opinions about the inaudibility of different components and general scoffing at high-end audio nuts. They really put it to the reader, though, when they made up their conclusion to Greenhill's article: "...the results demonstrated that while Monster Cable and 16-gauge lamp cord are both audibly different from and probably superior to 24-gauge wire, 16-gauge is good enough to be indistinguishable from Monster Cable when playing music. An esoteric cable would have to be demonstrably superior on 16-gauge wire... So what do our fifty hours of testing, scoring, comparing, and listening to speaker cables amount to? Only that 16-gauge lamp cord and Monster Cable are indistinguishable from each other with music and seem to be superior to the 24-gauge wire commonly sold or given away as 'speaker cable.' Remember, however, that it was a measurable characteristic--higher resistance per foot --that made 24-gauge wire sound different from the other cables. If the cable runs were only 6 instead of thirty feet, the overall cable resistances would have been lower and our test would probably have found no audible differences between the three cables." ******************************************************************** excuse me, but I can't hold it in any more HAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHAHA OK, I can type some more now.. ******************************************************************** Careful readers of the article as printed will know that this conclusion does not present an accurate description of the data that was gathered. Even more interesting, Stereo Review's editors entirely created that ending. Author Greenhill's original manuscript has none of the offensive language that eventually appeared. Here's how he put it: "The striking outcome is that the panel accurately heard and named speaker cables in 5 out of 6 comparisons. . . The listeners felt pleased after this listening test battery: they had heard real differences ! After 50 hours of testing, scoring, comparing, and just plain listening, they were exhausted but felt accomplished. Both Monster Cable and twenty-four gauge wire could be heard reliably under double blind conditions." Big difference, wouldn't you say? Yet Greenhill's original actually allows for two different interpretations of the data. One measure of the reliability of a series of identifications is known as chi square, or binomial, statistical analysis. Using this method, a prediction is made as to the probability of the identifications having occurred by chance. Another measure of reliability, one commonly used in psychoacoustic experiments, is the 75% rule: if a subject's identification (of the speaker cable, in this instance) is correct more than 75% of the time, the difference identified will almost certainly be audible-- presumably to most people, most of the time. When Stereo Review wrote their conclusions, they essentially threw out all the tests where a subject was unable to identify the cables more than 75% of the time; Greenhill, on the other hand, was impressed that his group of 11 listeners could pick, with better than 999 to 1 odds, between 16 gauge zipcord, 24 gauge zipcord, and 11.5 gauge Monster Cable (using the chi square method of statistical analysis). Greenhill's original report, which is three times the length of the published one, makes it possible to evaluate the two methods fairly. It's certainly possible to distinguish between 24-gauge and either 16-gauge or Monster: one can reliably distinguish between 16-gauge and Monster (using the best signal, pink noise), but the differences are right at the edge of audibility so that many listeners won't notice them, and the most acute listeners will. I think it's unfortunate that more of the original report didn't make it into print, instead being sacrificed to SR's editorial viewpoints. Greenhill and his friends from the Audiophile Society (who supplied 8 of the listeners for the cable test) have tried several double-blind listening tests, and none of them have come up with positive results (ie, reliable identification of the components in question). Here they finally achieve success in the form of interesting results, but those results are obscured by the time the report makes it to print. The first interesting result is that the listening panels preconceptions of cable performance had a large effect of perceived differences between cables *when they knew* which cables were in use. Second, differences were still perceived in double-blind testing, but to a much lesser degree. Third, panel members were surprised that the differences between the cables were so subtle and difficult to distinguish. Fourth, the performance of different panel members varied widely: there was one truely amazing "ear" amongst them, and four very good ones. Fifth, differences between very similar cable (none of them using exotic materials or cable geometry in their construction) could still be reliably picked out, even when (in one trial) the resistances of the different wires were artificially matched using a potentiometer. Sixth, pink noise is a better test signal for discrimination than the choral music selection used (not necessarily all music). With a list of positive results such as this, it really makes you wonder why SR chose to emphasize only the negative. Much can be learned from the coverage afforded the cable article. Stereo Review has used Larry Greenhill's article by distorting it to represent their well-established editorial positions. IAR has used it to draw attention to it's role as savior of the consumer and of the high-end industry. TAS has capitalized on the humor possibilities, and taken the opportunity to attack Julian Hirsch (Hearse)- -at whose feet I feel this matter is unjustly laid. Hans Fantel, who must have read only the conclusions and not the data, used the article to justify his hitherto- announced scorn for esoterica. The Wall Street Journal seemingly has no axe to grind, but Gregory Sandow has used their pages to not unfairly promote The Absolute Sound, for whom he also writes. Significant harm has been done, however. Truly esoteric speaker cables (of which New Monster Cable is definitely not one) have been maligned in the eyes of that portion of SR's 550,00 readers who are not intellectually discriminating-- and without even being tested. The cause of "scientific" testing has not been helped; here was a successful test which earned its author and participants not fame, but infamy. Larry Greenhill's position in the high-end community has been changed substantially , at least in the short run. Several manufacturers now refuse to talk to him, and a magazine he works for (High Performance Review) has even been denied the opportunity to review a certain manufacturer's products. Attacked by all manner of underground magazine (whose ranks he once felt a part of), Larry has hardly known which end was up these last few months. On the other hand, he has only himself to thank. He did sign off on the article as printed, with adequate time for review, although there was a lot of pressure to accept the changes suggested by SR's editors. His personal opinion is that it would be wise for him (and for others who write for those magazines whose primary interest is attracting advertisements) to be most careful about what they approve for publication. In keeping with Larry Greenhill's desire that the whole issue go away, we close our review of the cable article situation with a plea for a more objective stance from Stereo Review, who had the chance to expose as a success this attempt to differentiate between slightly varying components, and a plea for the many more tests such as the one carried out by Greenhill. Well, that's it. Lunch time..... Mark Nevar